Elsevier

Addictive Behaviors

Volume 26, Issue 2, March–April 2001, Pages 237-251
Addictive Behaviors

Subtypes within the precontemplation stage of adolescent smoking acquisition

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0306-4603(00)00104-0Get rights and content

Abstract

The present study examined the existence of three possible subtypes within the precontemplation stage of smoking acquisition: committers, immotives, and progressives. The sample of the study included young people from six European countries (mean age=13 years) who had never smoked regularly (n=21 535). A cross-sectional design was used to assess cognitive determinants of smoking behavior: attitudes towards smoking, perceived social influences, and self-efficacy to remain a nonsmoker. Adolescents within the various stages of smoking acquisition were compared with regard to standardized T scores on these risk factors. The results showed that adolescents in the three subgroups of precontemplation differed from each other on every cognitive determinant tested, revealing a higher risk to start smoking among progressives than among immotives. Immotives revealed a higher risk to start smoking than committers. It is concluded that the use of subtypes within acquisition precontemplation in research on adolescent smoking may be a promising tool for investigating the initiation continuum and for improving the quality of both the implementation and evaluation of prevention programs.

Introduction

Although the stages of change construct (SCC; Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983) was originally developed to study adult smoking cessation within the transtheoretical model of change (TTM; DiClemente & Prochaska, 1982, DiClemente et al., 1991, Prochaska & DiClemente, 1983), it has also been applied to adolescent smoking cessation and acquisition Elder et al., 1990, Kelley et al., 1999, Pallonen, 1998, Pallonen et al., 1998, Stern et al., 1987, Werch & DiClemente, 1994. The TTM of smoking acquisition can be regarded as the mirror image of the cessation model (Stern et al., 1987). The SCC distinguishes three stages of smoking acquisition (Pallonen et al., 1998), defined as acquisition precontemplation (no intentions to start smoking in the next 6 months), acquisition contemplation (thinking about starting to smoke in the next 6 months), and acquisition preparation (thinking about starting to smoke in the next 30 days).

Especially among early adolescents, a large majority of the population is in the precontemplation stage of smoking acquisition. The prevalence of acquisition precontemplators within the nonsmoking adolescent population has been found to be over 90% Aveyard et al., 1999, Pallonen et al., 1998. Since a large percentage of adolescents eventually will start smoking, the precontemplating group might be diverse with regard to predictors of smoking initiation. The possibility to provide finer discriminations makes identification of subtypes within a large group of precontemplators desirable (Crittenden, Manfredi, Lacey, Warnecke, & Parsons, 1994). Finer discriminations within acquisition precontemplators enable the possibility to provide accurate health education messages to a target group of nonsmoking adolescents.

Identification of possible subgroups within the precontemplation stage has been the subject of recent research on adult smoking cessation Crittenden et al., 1994, Dijkstra et al., 1997, Dijkstra et al., 1998, Norman et al., 2000, Velicer et al., 1995. Two clearly interpretable subgroups within precontemplation have been identified. Velicer et al. (1995) identified a subgroup of precontemplators ‘clearly ready to move to the contemplation stage’ that was labeled ‘progressives.’ A subgroup of precontemplating smokers who plan not to quit within the next 5 years has been labeled ‘immotives’ (Dijkstra et al., 1997; Velicer et al., 1995). Consequently, immotives are thought to be more remote from contemplation than the progressive subgroup. Norman et al. (2000) recently replicated and externally validated the existence of these subtypes within a representative sample of adult smokers. Perhaps the distinction between immotives and progressives could also be made for the acquisition stages of adolescent smoking.

A third possible subgroup within acquisition precontemplation may consist of adolescents who have a strong commitment not to smoke. The concept of commitment to nonsmoking, labeled as ‘non-susceptibility,’ is introduced by Pierce, Farkas, Evans, & Gilpin (1995). The move to start smoking may be influenced by the absence of a firm decision not to smoke Choi et al., 1997, Pierce et al., 1996, Unger et al., 1997. Measuring the intention to start smoking at some time in the future might not distinguish the children who have really thought consciously about being a nonsmoker from those who have not rationally decided not to smoke. Where immotives also plan never to start smoking, but lack the strong decision not to smoke, adolescents who are firmly committed to nonsmoking (‘committers’) might be more remote from the contemplation stage than immotives.

Over the past decades, much effort has been put into explaining and predicting adolescent smoking initiation Conrad et al., 1992, Tyas & Pederson, 1998. In their review of theories of adolescent substance use, Petraitis, Flay, & Miller (1995) conclude that attitudinal, social, and self-efficacy components are useful motivational (proximal) determinants to predict and explain adolescent smoking behavior. All other levels of influence are believed to exert little effect on adolescent substance use unless they first affect these substance-specific cognitions (Petraitis et al., 1995). For example, the attitude–social influence–efficacy (ASE) model De Vries et al., 1988, De Vries & Mudde, 1998 proved to be of value in explaining and predicting adolescent smoking behavior De Vries et al., 1995, De Vries et al., 1988, Mudde et al., 1999.

Findings in several studies suggest that the stages of change not only differ because of differences in intentions to smoke, but also because of differences in the impact of the cognitive determinants of smoking (Velicer, Norman, Fava, & Prochaska, 1999). Cognitive determinants, referred to as dynamic constructs in the TTM, have been shown to differ between the various stages of change of smoking cessation Prochaska & DiClemente, 1984, Prochaska et al., 1991, Velicer et al., 1999. Findings in studies using the ASE model have resembled these differences, despite the use of different cognitive constructs De Vries & Backbier, 1994, De Vries et al., 1998, Dijkstra et al., 1996.

The objective of the present paper is to investigate the identification of the concepts of commitment, immotivation, and progressiveness as different subtypes within the precontemplation stage of adolescent smoking acquisition. The usefulness of a more detailed description of the acquisition precontemplation stage was tested by assessing differences between the various subgroups regarding cognitive predictors of smoking behavior. It was hypothesized that adolescents in the commitment phase would score higher than immotives with regard to nonsmoking attitudes, perceive less negative social influences with regard to smoking, and have higher self-efficacy with regard to refraining from smoking. Immotives are expected to show the same pattern when compared to progressives.

Section snippets

Participants and recruitment

In the present study, seventh graders in a large international sample of European adolescents (n=23 304) were measured cross-sectionally. This study is part of a larger, ongoing prospective study of the European Smoking Prevention Framework Approach (ESFA) project (De Vries et al., in preparation). Seven sites in six European Union member states (Finland, Denmark, the UK, the Netherlands, Barcelona (Spain), Madrid (Spain), and Portugal) participate in this project. Data were collected at school,

Current smoking status

Adolescents were to pick a statement that best described them out of a set of specific smoking-related statements. Responses were cross-validated using an algorithm, consisting of concepts measuring current smoking and lifetime smoking. A primary measure of regular use is smoking, on the average, at least once a week or with greater frequency Flay, 1993, Sussman et al., 1995, US Department of Health and Human Services, 1994. Consequently, smoking behavior was classified into four stages:

  • 1.

    Never

Demographics and experimenting behavior within stages

The distribution of adolescents over the five stage groups — committers, immotives, progressives, contemplators, and preparators — did differ significantly for gender, age, country, and experimental smoking behavior (Table 2). Girls seemed to be more clearly progressing towards smoking than boys. There were hardly any differences in age distribution between the groups. In comparison to the other sites in our study, Barcelonese adolescents tended not to commit themselves to nonsmoking, while the

Discussion

This study investigated the existence of subgroups within the precontemplation stage of adolescent smoking acquisition, by assessing differences between the various potential subgroups regarding cognitive predictors of smoking behavior. Usually, the precontemplating group is very large in an adolescent population, as was confirmed by the present study: 90% of the total sample was in acquisition precontemplation. Relatively few individuals are in acquisition contemplation and acquisition

Acknowledgements

This study was financed with the assistance of the European Community, Fund for Tobacco Research and Information [Regulation (EC) No 2427/93].

We are grateful to the ESFA National Project Managers Klavs Holm, Riku Lehtovuori, Karin Janssen, Paulo Vitória, Carles Ariza, Lourdes Fresnillo, Anne Fielder, and Max Maqbul Mughal for their contribution in the data-gathering process.

References (47)

  • G.J Norman et al.

    Cluster subtypes within stage of change in a representative sample of smokers

    Addictive Behaviors

    (2000)
  • U.E Pallonen

    Transtheoretical measures for adolescent and adult smokers: similarities and differences

    Preventive Medicine

    (1998)
  • U.E Pallonen et al.

    Stages of acquisition and cessation for adolescent smoking: an empirical integration

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1998)
  • R.A Stern et al.

    Stages of adolescent cigarette smoking acquisition: measurement and sample profiles

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1987)
  • J.B Unger et al.

    Identification of adolescents at risk for smoking initiation: validation of a measure of susceptibility

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1997)
  • W.F Velicer et al.

    An empirical typology of subjects within stages of change

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1995)
  • W.F Velicer et al.

    Testing 40 predictions from the transtheoretical model

    Addictive Behaviors

    (1999)
  • P Aveyard et al.

    Cluster randomised controlled trial of expert system based on the transtheoretical (“stages of change”) model for smoking prevention and cessation in schools

    British Medical Journal

    (1999)
  • K.M Conrad et al.

    Why children start smoking cigarettes: predictors of onset

    British Journal of Addiction

    (1992)
  • H De Vries et al.

    A Dutch social influence smoking prevention approach for vocational school students

    Health Education Research

    (1994)
  • H De Vries et al.

    The impact of social influences in the context of attitude, self-efficacy, intention and previous behaviour as predictors of smoking onset

    Journal of Applied Social Psychology

    (1995)
  • H De Vries et al.

    Self-efficacy: the third factor besides attitude and subjective norm as a predictor of behavior intention

    Health Education Research

    (1988)
  • H De Vries et al.

    From determinants of smoking behaviour to the implications for a prevention programme

    Health Education Research

    (1986)
  • Cited by (61)

    • Exposure to peers who smoke moderates the association between sports participation and cigarette smoking behavior among non-White adolescents

      2012, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      This measure has been demonstrated to have good reliability (test–retest Cohen's κ 0.56–0.75) (Brener et al., 2002; Zullig, Pun, Patton, & Ubbes, 2006) and has been widely used in studies of adolescent sports participation and tobacco and other substance use behaviors (Lisha & Sussman, 2010; Mays, Gatti, & Thompson, 2011). Exposure to peers who smoke cigarettes was assessed using four items that are widely used in research on risk factors for adolescent smoking (Castrucci, Gerlach, Kaufman, & Orleans, 2002; Kremers, Mudde, & de Vries, 2001). Questions assessed whether participants' best friend, best male friend, best female friend, and girlfriend/boyfriend, partner, or spouse smoked.

    • Cluster subtypes appropriate for preventing postpartum smoking relapse

      2012, Addictive Behaviors
      Citation Excerpt :

      The cluster analysis presented here replicates those obtained in previous studies examining adolescent acquisition (Velicer et al., 2007, 2009), and German postpartum reacquisition (Thyrian et al., 2006). With unique populations such as adolescents who have not yet started to smoke and postpartum women who have quit for pregnancy, subdivision of the acquisition-precontemplation stage has revealed some distinct differences on which tailored interventions may be applied (Dijkstra et al., 1997; Kremers et al., 2001; Norman et al., 2000; Pallonen et al., 1998; Velicer et al., 2007). In these very different populations, the same four clusters have been identified within aPC stage.

    • Health education and health promotion: Key concepts and exemplary evidence to support them

      2018, Principles and Concepts of Behavioral Medicine: A Global Handbook
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text