The authors, Cifuentes MP, Rodriguez-Villamizar LA, Rojas-Botero ML, et al [1], present an article that, owing to a lack of rigor in the creation and application of ethno-racial categories, ends up employing an analysis method that, although intended to allow proving inequalities, ends up disguising or attenuating them.
Raj Bhopal published a seminal article in this journal in 2004 demonstrating the importance of ethnicity and race variables in epidemiology and public health. Bhopal pointed out that, at a minimum, researchers should explain their understanding of the concepts of race or ethnicity and the classification they use, even more so when we know that they need development in terms of geographic specificity, scope, and precision for different contexts [2]. Similar recommendations are made by Janeth Mosquera in her analysis about the use of the ethnic-racial category in the research published by the three most important scientific journals of Public Health in Colombia [3].
The paper does not present a comprehensive and helpful description of the categories that assist the reader in understanding the ethnic-racial composition of the Colombian population and correctly analyze the regularly available data for public health surveillance. The Colombian surveillance system employs the census ethnic-racial categories. Among these Census categories, the "white-mestizo" used by the authors is not defined and thus is not used for public health surveill...
The authors, Cifuentes MP, Rodriguez-Villamizar LA, Rojas-Botero ML, et al [1], present an article that, owing to a lack of rigor in the creation and application of ethno-racial categories, ends up employing an analysis method that, although intended to allow proving inequalities, ends up disguising or attenuating them.
Raj Bhopal published a seminal article in this journal in 2004 demonstrating the importance of ethnicity and race variables in epidemiology and public health. Bhopal pointed out that, at a minimum, researchers should explain their understanding of the concepts of race or ethnicity and the classification they use, even more so when we know that they need development in terms of geographic specificity, scope, and precision for different contexts [2]. Similar recommendations are made by Janeth Mosquera in her analysis about the use of the ethnic-racial category in the research published by the three most important scientific journals of Public Health in Colombia [3].
The paper does not present a comprehensive and helpful description of the categories that assist the reader in understanding the ethnic-racial composition of the Colombian population and correctly analyze the regularly available data for public health surveillance. The Colombian surveillance system employs the census ethnic-racial categories. Among these Census categories, the "white-mestizo" used by the authors is not defined and thus is not used for public health surveillance. Besides, the authors do not define "white-mestizo."
The authors employ an official database published by Colombian national sanitary authorities and provide a link to it. However, as systematic users of the cited source, we must point out that three of the model's explanatory variables are not available on the authors-cited open data platform. Therefore, it is necessary to warn the reader that micro-data on social security affiliation, household's socioeconomic stratum, and area of residence are not available. Consequently, the results are not reproductible with publicly available data. The authors should indicate how they accessed this data and the ethical implications if they exist.
Finally, it is crucial to recognize that the paper implicitly assumes homogeneity in the group's distribution, risk exposure, and events. Authors should discuss whether it is possible, with the available data, to have a syndemic approach, as proposed early on by Bambra and collaborators [4]. This kind of analysis accounts for structural differences within the country and how ethnic groups relate to each other in the territories.
References
1. Cifuentes MP, Rodriguez-Villamizar LA, Rojas-Botero ML, et al. Socioeconomic inequalities associated with mortality for COVID-19 in Colombia: a cohort nationwide study. J Epidemiol Community Health 2021;75:610-615; DOI:10.1136/jech-2020-216275
2. Bhopal R. Glossary of terms relating to ethnicity and race: for reflection and debate. Journal of Epidemiology & Community Health 2004;58:441-445; DOI:10.1136/jech.2003.013466
3. Mosquera Becerra J. Unveiling what is said in the colombian public health journals about race and ethnicity. Rev.CS 2015;16:109-2; DOI: https://doi.org/10.18046/recs.i16.1939
4. Bambra C, Riordan R, Ford J, et al. The COVID-19 pandemic and health inequalities. J Epidemiol Community Health 2020;74:964-968; DOI:10.1136/jech-2020-214401
Frank’s essay contains several statements about 5G, its relation to the radiation protection science, and related to this, ICNIRP’s guidance and integrity more generally. ICNIRP considers this to be seriously inaccurate and in need of correction for the sake of both scientific accuracy and development of effective public health policy. However, due to journal word limits we must restrict our response to Frank’s misleading claims about ICNIRP’s integrity (for full response see https://www.icnirp.org/en/activities/news/index.html).
Frank’s essay accuses ICNIRP of unmanaged conflict of interests, and uses this accusation to attempt to throw doubt on ICNIRP’s scientific evaluations. However, ICNIRP has a very rigorous procedure to avoid conflicts of interest (https://www.icnirp.org/en/about-icnirp/commission/index.html), and Frank did not provide any evidence in support of his statement - he merely referred to ‘persistent allegations’ from the Swedish epidemiologist Lennart Hardell. For example, Frank repeats claims made by Hardell that “ICNIRP’s membership includes over-representation of vested interests, especially the giant multinational telecommunications firms who are heavily invested in the roll out of 5G systems internationally”, and no supporting evidence was provided by either author. To be clear, there are no industry r...
Frank’s essay contains several statements about 5G, its relation to the radiation protection science, and related to this, ICNIRP’s guidance and integrity more generally. ICNIRP considers this to be seriously inaccurate and in need of correction for the sake of both scientific accuracy and development of effective public health policy. However, due to journal word limits we must restrict our response to Frank’s misleading claims about ICNIRP’s integrity (for full response see https://www.icnirp.org/en/activities/news/index.html).
Frank’s essay accuses ICNIRP of unmanaged conflict of interests, and uses this accusation to attempt to throw doubt on ICNIRP’s scientific evaluations. However, ICNIRP has a very rigorous procedure to avoid conflicts of interest (https://www.icnirp.org/en/about-icnirp/commission/index.html), and Frank did not provide any evidence in support of his statement - he merely referred to ‘persistent allegations’ from the Swedish epidemiologist Lennart Hardell. For example, Frank repeats claims made by Hardell that “ICNIRP’s membership includes over-representation of vested interests, especially the giant multinational telecommunications firms who are heavily invested in the roll out of 5G systems internationally”, and no supporting evidence was provided by either author. To be clear, there are no industry representations within ICNIRP; people working for industry are not permitted to be ICNIRP members, and ICNIRP does not receive any funding or in-kind contributions from industry. Moving beyond funding, Frank claims that the “the most damning evidence adduced by Hardell is a table of the cross-appointments held by six members of the WHO Monograph Group, across five major international advisory panels on the health effects of non-ionising radiation”. However, this merely reflects the relatively small EMF research community together with the high public interest that has resulted in many national and international advisory boards. Frank does not tell the reader why this would in any way bias ICNIRP’s work. Thus, the essay’s accusations of industry influence are not only incorrect, but also lack appropriate scientific scholarship.
In conclusion, ICNIRP fully supports and contributes to critical debate related to the 5G rollout, but would like to stress the importance of approaching such debate with appropriate scientific scholarship in order to support effective, evidence-based public health measures that provide appropriate protection to the public. For ICNIRP it is imperative to avoid situations whereby personal interests of its members could affect the independence of ICNIRP’s guidance, and so has strong conflict of interest procedures, including transparent reporting of members’ declarations of interest.
Acknowledgements
Rodney Croft (1), Tania Cestari (2), Nigel Cridland (3), Akimasa Hirata (4), Guglielmo d'Inzeo (5), Anke Huss (6), Ken Karipidis (7), Carmela Marino (8), Sharon Miller (9), Gunnhild Oftedal (10), Tsutomu Okuno (11), Eric van Rongen (12), Martin Röösli (13), Soichi Watanabe (14).
Correspondence to: International Commission on Non-Ionizing Radiation Protection, Dr Gunde Ziegelberger, ICNIRP c/o BfS, Ingolstaedter Landstr. 1, 85764 Oberschleissheim, Germany. info@icnirp.org
Affiliations of ICNIRP Collaborators
1. ICNIRP and Australian Centre for Electromagnetic Bioeffects Research, Illawarra Health & Medical Research Institute, University of Wollongong, Australia
2. ICNIRP and Hospital de Clínicas de Porto Alegre, Brazil
3. ICNIRP and Public Health England, United Kingdom
4. ICNIRP and Nagoya Institute of Technology, Japan
5. ICNIRP and La Sapienza University Rome, Italy
6. ICNIRP and Institute for Risk Assessment, Utrecht University, The Netherlands
7. ICNIRP and Australian Radiation Protection and Nuclear Safety Agency (ARPANSA), Australia
8. ICNIRP and Agency for New Technologies, Energy and Sustainable Economic Development (ENEA), Italy
9. ICNIRP
10. ICNIRP and Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU), Norway
11. ICNIRP
12. ICNIRP and Health Council, The Netherlands
13. ICNIRP and Swiss Tropical and Public Health Institute, Basel, Switzerland
14. ICNIRP and National Institute of Information and Communications Technology (NICT), Japan
Hengartner et al. conducted a meta-analysis on suicide risk with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) and other new-generation antidepressants in adults (1). Although the pooled relative risks (RRs) of SSRI for suicide risk including suicide and suicide attempt in patients with depression and in patients with all indications did not reach the level of significance, the pooled RR (95% confidence intervals [CIs]) of any new-generation antidepressant for suicide risk in patients with depression and in patients with all indications were 1.29 (1.06-1.57) and 1.45 (1.23-1.70), respectively. The authors presented information on the different suicide risk between SSRI and other new-generation antidepressants , and I present additional information regarding the relationship.
First, Sharma et al. conducted a meta-analysis on the association of SSRI and serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors with suicidality and other mental indicators (2). Although the pooled odds ratios (ORs) of antidepressant treatment for suicidality and aggression did not reach the level of significance in adults, the pooed ORs (95% CIs) of antidepressant treatment for suicidality and aggression were 2.39 (1.31-4.33) and 2.79 (1.62-4.81) in children/adolescents. The suicide risk differed in different generations, and suicide risk estimation should be conducted by stratification with generation and type of anti-depressants.
Second, Hengartner and Plöderl reported that odds ratios (OR...
Hengartner et al. conducted a meta-analysis on suicide risk with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) and other new-generation antidepressants in adults (1). Although the pooled relative risks (RRs) of SSRI for suicide risk including suicide and suicide attempt in patients with depression and in patients with all indications did not reach the level of significance, the pooled RR (95% confidence intervals [CIs]) of any new-generation antidepressant for suicide risk in patients with depression and in patients with all indications were 1.29 (1.06-1.57) and 1.45 (1.23-1.70), respectively. The authors presented information on the different suicide risk between SSRI and other new-generation antidepressants , and I present additional information regarding the relationship.
First, Sharma et al. conducted a meta-analysis on the association of SSRI and serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors with suicidality and other mental indicators (2). Although the pooled odds ratios (ORs) of antidepressant treatment for suicidality and aggression did not reach the level of significance in adults, the pooed ORs (95% CIs) of antidepressant treatment for suicidality and aggression were 2.39 (1.31-4.33) and 2.79 (1.62-4.81) in children/adolescents. The suicide risk differed in different generations, and suicide risk estimation should be conducted by stratification with generation and type of anti-depressants.
Second, Hengartner and Plöderl reported that odds ratios (ORs) (95% confidence intervals [CIs]) of antidepressant treatment for suicides and suicide attempts were 2.83 (1.13-9.67) and 2.38 (1.63-3.61), respectively (3). By using the same database, Kaminski et al. reported that ORs (95% CIs) of antidepressant treatment for suicides and suicide attempts were 1.98 (0.71-5.50) and 1.63 (1.09-2.43), respectively (4). In case of rare events, the level of significance in OR differed by applying different analytical approaches. This means that marginal level of significance should be verified by further studies, and a meta-analysis with high quality of papers is recommended to confirm the association.
References
1. Hengartner MP, Amendola S, Kaminski JA, et al. Suicide risk with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and other new-generation antidepressants in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. J Epidemiol Community Health 2021 Mar 8. doi: 10.1136/jech-2020-214611. [Epub ahead of print]
2. Sharma T, Guski LS, Freund N, et al. Suicidality and aggression during antidepressant treatment: systematic review and meta-analyses based on clinical study reports. BMJ 2016;352:i65.
3. Hengartner MP, Plöderl M. Newer-generation antidepressants and suicide risk in randomized controlled trials: A re-analysis of the FDA database. Psychother Psychosom 2019;88(4):247-248.
4. Kaminski JA, Bschor T. Antidepressants and suicidality: A re-analysis of the re-analysis. J Affect Disord 2020;266:95-99.
We read with interest, intrigue and concern the findings reported in this short report. if the findings are validated from larger and multicentric data this may have huge implications in the way we trace and isolate the COVID-19 contacts. Pre symptomatic transmission from index cases 5 days prior to the onset of symptoms is a huge logistical nightmare in terms of containment strategies. This would imply at practical impossibility and futility of these strategy especially in setting of cluster or community transmission. This also highlights the virtues of basic but universal measures like physical distancing, hygiene and use of mask at all times under specific settings.
The question “Why does Russia have such high cardiovascular (CV) mortality rates?”1 can be answered by a pathologist who practiced during the Soviet time.2 Since then, the quality of post mortem examinations has decreased especially during the 1990s: autopsies were sometimes made perfunctorily. The deterioration in anatomic pathology and the health care in general during the 1990s coincided with the increase in the registered CV mortality. A tendency to over-diagnose CV diseases is generally known to exist also for people dying at home and not undergoing autopsy. If a cause of death is not entirely clear, it has been usual to write on a death certificate: “Ischemic heart disease with cardiac insufficiency” or a similar formulation.2 Concerning the relatively high CV mortality in Russia, it should be commented that irregular treatment of hypertension,3 diabetes and other chronic diseases continues to be a problem. Considering the above, the differences between Norwegian and Russian cohorts1 can be better understood. The levels of serum lipids were comparable between Russia and Norway being slightly higher in the latter possibly due to better nutrition. Interestingly, N-terminal pro-b-type natriuretic peptide (NT-proBNP), high-sensitivity cardiac troponin T (hs-cTnT), and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP) were higher in Russia.1 It can be reasonably assumed that average levels of these markers inversely correlate with a nation’s health reflected by the life expecta...
The question “Why does Russia have such high cardiovascular (CV) mortality rates?”1 can be answered by a pathologist who practiced during the Soviet time.2 Since then, the quality of post mortem examinations has decreased especially during the 1990s: autopsies were sometimes made perfunctorily. The deterioration in anatomic pathology and the health care in general during the 1990s coincided with the increase in the registered CV mortality. A tendency to over-diagnose CV diseases is generally known to exist also for people dying at home and not undergoing autopsy. If a cause of death is not entirely clear, it has been usual to write on a death certificate: “Ischemic heart disease with cardiac insufficiency” or a similar formulation.2 Concerning the relatively high CV mortality in Russia, it should be commented that irregular treatment of hypertension,3 diabetes and other chronic diseases continues to be a problem. Considering the above, the differences between Norwegian and Russian cohorts1 can be better understood. The levels of serum lipids were comparable between Russia and Norway being slightly higher in the latter possibly due to better nutrition. Interestingly, N-terminal pro-b-type natriuretic peptide (NT-proBNP), high-sensitivity cardiac troponin T (hs-cTnT), and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP) were higher in Russia.1 It can be reasonably assumed that average levels of these markers inversely correlate with a nation’s health reflected by the life expectancy at birth. Indeed, elevated C-reactive protein is known to be associated with various inflammatory conditions. The natriuretic peptide (NP) plays an important role by opposing the vasoconstriction and sodium retention. A plasma NP elevation was found in essential hypertension, decreasing with effective antihypertensive therapy.4 Hs-cTnT is a biomarker for myocardial damage; but other conditions are also associated with its enhanced level: diabetes, COPD, decreased renal function, anaemia etc.5 The insufficient access to modern healthcare,1 higher consumption of alcohol and cigarettes in Russia vs. Norway (the data can be found in Wikipedia), as well as relatively poor quality of alcohol sold in Russia,6 have probably contributed to a higher morbidity. In conclusion, the valuable results by Dr. Iakunchykova and co-workers1 should motivate further search for associations between NT-proBNP, hs-cTnT and other markers with various pathological conditions.
REFERENCES
1. Iakunchykova O, Averina M, Wilsgaard T, et al. Why does Russia have such high cardiovascular mortality rates? Comparisons of blood–based biomarkers with Norway implicate non-ischaemic cardiac damage. J Epidemiol Community Health 2020;74:698–704.
2. Jargin SV. Cardiovascular mortality trends in Russia: possible mechanisms. Nat Rev Cardiol 2015;12:740.
3. Roberts B, Stickley A, Balabanova D, et al. The persistence of irregular treatment of hypertension in the former Soviet Union. J Epidemiol Community Health 2012;66:1079–82.
4. Hu W, Zhou PH, Zhang XB, Xu CG, Wang W. Plasma concentrations of adrenomedullin and natriuretic peptides in patients with essential hypertension. Exp Ther Med 2015;9:1901–908.
5. Wu W, Li DX, Wang Q, et al. Relationship between high-sensitivity cardiac troponin T and the prognosis of elderly inpatients with non-acute coronary syndromes. Clin Interv Aging 2018;13:1091–8.
6. Jargin SV. Vodka vs. Fortified Wine in Russia: Retrospective View. Alcohol Alcohol 2015;50:624–5.
We read with great interest the report from Hamer and colleagues that examined the hypothesis that associations between television (TV) viewing and mortality from heart disease (HD) are due to confounding (1). They employed a negative control approach (2) and report evidence of associations between TV viewing and HD mortality (HR=1.09 [1.06, 1.12] per 1 hr/day increase in TV) and accidental deaths (the negative control outcome; HR=1.06 [0.98, 1.15]) after adjusting for age, sex, smoking, education, and prevalent HD (1)
The positive association between TV and accidental deaths was interpreted as evidence that the TV-HD mortality association was due to confounding. Although key study limitations were noted including a small number of accidental deaths and limited adjustment for confounding, the authors concluded that “observed associations between TV and HD are likely to be driven by confounding”. Although we agree that confounding is a worrisome threat to the internal validity of epidemiologic studies, we believe that the conclusion in the Hamer report is overstated.
A critical additional strategy to understand bias due to confounding, one that was not employed in the current study, is to examine relevant results from published studies conducted in different study populations using different methods. (2) We previously reported results in two studies that examined associations for accidental deaths and HD mortality with TV viewing (3) and leisure-time sitti...
We read with great interest the report from Hamer and colleagues that examined the hypothesis that associations between television (TV) viewing and mortality from heart disease (HD) are due to confounding (1). They employed a negative control approach (2) and report evidence of associations between TV viewing and HD mortality (HR=1.09 [1.06, 1.12] per 1 hr/day increase in TV) and accidental deaths (the negative control outcome; HR=1.06 [0.98, 1.15]) after adjusting for age, sex, smoking, education, and prevalent HD (1)
The positive association between TV and accidental deaths was interpreted as evidence that the TV-HD mortality association was due to confounding. Although key study limitations were noted including a small number of accidental deaths and limited adjustment for confounding, the authors concluded that “observed associations between TV and HD are likely to be driven by confounding”. Although we agree that confounding is a worrisome threat to the internal validity of epidemiologic studies, we believe that the conclusion in the Hamer report is overstated.
A critical additional strategy to understand bias due to confounding, one that was not employed in the current study, is to examine relevant results from published studies conducted in different study populations using different methods. (2) We previously reported results in two studies that examined associations for accidental deaths and HD mortality with TV viewing (3) and leisure-time sitting (predominantly TV viewing) (4), but these findings were not cited in the Hamer report. Our studies each examined 3-5 times more accidental deaths than the Hamer report, and adjusted for a much larger number of confounding factors. (3,4) We found significant positive associations with TV-HD mortality, but no evidence of association between accidental deaths and greater TV time (HR=1.01 [0.62, 1.64]; 7+ vs. < 1 hr/day) (3) or leisure-time sitting (HR=0.91 [0.76, 1.10]; 6+ vs. < 3 hr/d). (4) In context of the negative control outcome framework, our results provide no evidence that previously observed HD associations, or associations with several other causes of death, were due only to confounding. (3,4)
Many studies have reported positive associations between disease/mortality outcomes and TV, a prevalent leisure-time behavior that, as Hamer and colleagues note, is likely to displace time spent in more healthful physically active pursuits. We strongly support efforts to better understand these relationships, including careful consideration of bias and threats to validity. As we do so, it is critical that we consider the broad range of information available before drawing strong conclusions based on a single study.
References
1. Hamer M, Ding D, Chau J, Duncan MJ, Stamatakis E. Association between TV viewing and heart disease mortality: observational study using negative control outcome. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health. 2020:jech-2019-212739.
2. Pearce N, Vandenbroucke JP, Lawlor DA. Causal Inference in Environmental Epidemiology: Old and New Approaches. Epidemiology. 2019;30(3):311-316.
3. Keadle SK, Moore SC, Sampson JN, Xiao Q, Albanes D, Matthews CE. Causes of Death Associated With Prolonged TV Viewing: NIH-AARP Diet and Health Study. American Journal of Preventive Medicine. 2015;49(6):811-821.
4. Patel AV, Maliniak ML, Rees-Punia E, Matthews CE, Gapstur SM. Prolonged Leisure Time Spent Sitting in Relation to Cause-Specific Mortality in a Large US Cohort. American Journal of Epidemiology. 2018;187(10):2151-2158.
Dear Editor,
The authors of “Effects of physical activity calorie equivalent food labelling to reduce food selection and consumption: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies” make a strong claim that PACE food labeling can increase consumer consciousness of calorie consumption and therefore caloric reduction, but perhaps this concept deters true understanding of "healthful eating" and may have larger health implications for those with disordered eating.
There is a growing knowledge that that not all calories are created equal. Different foods may not only have different effects on hunger and satiety but also insulin production, gut microbiome interactions, and de novo lipogenesis in the liver (1). While not all consumers need this level of understanding, but without a basic acknowledgement of food’s qualities- like fats, fiber, sugar, ect- the consumer is lead to believe that calories are the most important determinant in what makes food “healthful.” With PACE food labeling, a consumer is led to believe that an ice cream cone and a handful of nuts, both of which could amount to 200 calories, are “equal.” However, in this comparison, only the nuts are possibly advantageous to people with diabetes and cardiovascular disease (2).
Stripping foods down to solely their caloric energy through PACE food labeling could inadvertently foster unhealthy relationships with food. As stated in the article, PACE labeling could be use...
Dear Editor,
The authors of “Effects of physical activity calorie equivalent food labelling to reduce food selection and consumption: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies” make a strong claim that PACE food labeling can increase consumer consciousness of calorie consumption and therefore caloric reduction, but perhaps this concept deters true understanding of "healthful eating" and may have larger health implications for those with disordered eating.
There is a growing knowledge that that not all calories are created equal. Different foods may not only have different effects on hunger and satiety but also insulin production, gut microbiome interactions, and de novo lipogenesis in the liver (1). While not all consumers need this level of understanding, but without a basic acknowledgement of food’s qualities- like fats, fiber, sugar, ect- the consumer is lead to believe that calories are the most important determinant in what makes food “healthful.” With PACE food labeling, a consumer is led to believe that an ice cream cone and a handful of nuts, both of which could amount to 200 calories, are “equal.” However, in this comparison, only the nuts are possibly advantageous to people with diabetes and cardiovascular disease (2).
Stripping foods down to solely their caloric energy through PACE food labeling could inadvertently foster unhealthy relationships with food. As stated in the article, PACE labeling could be used to “help the public understand what a calorie means and therefore more able to decide whether the calories are ‘worth it.’” Through directing consumers to regard food as just calories to burn off through exercise, instead of fuel for a person’s body, perhaps consumers will come to regard food as punishment instead of its role as nourishment. Those who could be most affected from this way of thinking are the 30 million individuals living in the US with eating disorders (3,4). While obesity does affect more individuals than eating disorders, this is a population of individuals that should be considered when suggesting policymakers to consider implementation of this labeling system. Eating disorders are characterized by a pervasive thought pattern concerning both weight and food so implementing a labeling system that emphasizes food purging through the form of exercise can be detrimental to the mental health of these unique populations (5). Eating disorders have the highest mortality rate of any mental illness and 1 in 5 anorexia deaths are by suicide (6,7). Not only could PACE food labeling adversely affect those with eating disorders, but through fostering unhealthful relationships with food, they could potentially exacerbate the issue. On the other hand, reducing the risk and/or actual prevalence of obesity and all its related ailments, including psychological stress, is a worthy endeavor.
In conclusion, while the authors of this article review a novel and potentially helpful way to combat the growing obesity epidemic, further studies need to be done to compare PACE against current public health education programs and to investigate potential harm to those with eating disorders prior to policy making.
1) Mozaffarian, D.(2017). Foods, obesity, and diabetes—are all calories created equal?, Nutrition Reviews, Issue Suppl_1 Volume 75, Pages 19–31
2) Allen, L. (2008). Priority Areas for Research on the Intake, Composition, and Health Effects of Tree Nuts and Peanuts, The Journal of Nutrition, Volume 138, Issue 9, Pages 1763S–1765S
3) Hudson, J. I., Hiripi, E., Pope, H. G., & Kessler, R. C. (2007). The prevalence and correlates of eating disorders in the national comorbidity survey replication. Biological Psychiatry, 61(3), 348–358.
4) Le Grange, D., Swanson, S. A., Crow, S. J., & Merikangas, K. R. (2012). Eating disorder not otherwise specified presentation in the US population. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 45(5), 711-718.
5) Dell'Osso, L., Abelli, M., Carpita, B., Pini, S., Castellini, G., Carmassi, C., & Ricca, V. (2016). Historical evolution of the concept of anorexia nervosa and relationships with orthorexia nervosa, autism, and obsessive-compulsive spectrum. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment, 12, 1651–1660.
6) Smink, F. E., van Hoeken, D., & Hoek, H. W. (2012). Epidemiology of eating disorders: Incidence, prevalence and mortality rates. Current Psychiatry Reports,14, 406-414.
7) Arcelus, J., Mitchell, A. J., Wales, J., & Nielsen, S. (2011). Mortality rates in patients with anorexia nervosa and other eating disorders: a meta-analysis of 36 studies. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 724-731.
Much has been published in the news as of late about the effects of physical activity calorie equivalent (PACE) food labelling in order to reduce the nation’s calorie consumption. These labels aim to identify how many minutes of physical activity are required to burn off the calories in a particular food item. A systematic review and meta-analysis, by researchers at Loughborough University, found that food labelling may reduce the number of calories consumed compared with food that was not labelled or other types of food labelling (1).
This was supported by the UK Royal Society for Public Health which had already advocated for PACE to replace the current labelling system (2). Overall, it found this technique could lead to a reduction of 100 calories per day combined with an increase in physical activity.
Many nutritionists have been quick to criticise, stating that it loses sight of the fact that food goes beyond calories and is fundamental for social aspects of life (3). Additionally, the nutritional content of food might be neglected. For example, it might be easier to “burn off” a chocolate bar than something with much more nutritious such as nut butters or a banana. This could result in people picking the easier but not necessarily the “healthier option.” Digestion is complex and although foods such as nuts and oats might be high in calories, their content results in slower processing and digestion. This allows people to feel fuller fo...
Much has been published in the news as of late about the effects of physical activity calorie equivalent (PACE) food labelling in order to reduce the nation’s calorie consumption. These labels aim to identify how many minutes of physical activity are required to burn off the calories in a particular food item. A systematic review and meta-analysis, by researchers at Loughborough University, found that food labelling may reduce the number of calories consumed compared with food that was not labelled or other types of food labelling (1).
This was supported by the UK Royal Society for Public Health which had already advocated for PACE to replace the current labelling system (2). Overall, it found this technique could lead to a reduction of 100 calories per day combined with an increase in physical activity.
Many nutritionists have been quick to criticise, stating that it loses sight of the fact that food goes beyond calories and is fundamental for social aspects of life (3). Additionally, the nutritional content of food might be neglected. For example, it might be easier to “burn off” a chocolate bar than something with much more nutritious such as nut butters or a banana. This could result in people picking the easier but not necessarily the “healthier option.” Digestion is complex and although foods such as nuts and oats might be high in calories, their content results in slower processing and digestion. This allows people to feel fuller for longer and, thus, perhaps eat less (3). Therefore, PACE labelling might be too simplistic and ultimately might not be the ideal option for promoting a healthy balanced diet.
People of lower socioeconomic classes are most at risk of suffering from obesity and the subsequent health related consequences of this (4). It is these same people who are likely to have less disposable income to join gyms, buy and cook fresh, nutritious meals and are less likely to access health care. Public Health England should be focusing on making healthy foods more accessible and cheaper so that everyone has the option to incorporate foods such as fruits and vegetables into their diet.
Innovative ideas are needed to promote weight loss in the current obesity epidemic. However, more realistic steps are required to make sure that these are targeted at those who are most at risk.
References
1. Daley AJ, McGee E, Bayliss S, Coombe A and Parretti H. lEffects of physical activity calorie equivalent food labelling to reduce food selection and consumption: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomised controlled studies. J Epidemiol Community Health. Published Online First: 10 December 2019. doi: 10.1136/jech-2019-213216
2. Royal Society for Public Health. Introducing “activity equivalent” calorie labelling to tackle obesity. [Internet]. 2016. [Cited 2019 December 15]. Available from: https://www.rsph.org.uk/uploads/assets/uploaded/26deda5b-b3b7-4b15-a11be...
3. Overby NC, Sonestedt E, Laaksonen DE, Birgisdottir BE. Dietary fiber and the glycemic index: a background paper for the Nordic Nutrition Recommendations 2012. Food Nutr Res. 2013;57:10.3402/fnr.v57i0.20709. doi:10.3402/fnr.v57i0.20709
Davis and colleagues must be commended for their concern about cancer outcomes in patients with mental disorders and for acknowledging the poor quality of research.(1) However, their statement “pre-existing mental disorder have a higher odds of advanced stage cancer at diagnosis “ deserve comment.
Firstly, patients with mental disorders, as all vulnerable populations, have poor access to care, considering either quantitatively or qualitatively, even more for specialized care, whatever it could be. Accordingly, a vertical approach only targeting patients with mental disorders would only be a partial and symptomatic solution A root cause analysis is a prerequisite to expect fixing a systemic failure.
Secondly, the term “pre-existing mental disorder” must be questioned as tobacco and alcohol cause both mental health problems and cancer. There is robust and accumulating evidence that cigarette smoking is a causal risk factor for anxiety, depression and, even severe mental illness such as bipolar disorder.(2) Cessation is associated with reduced depression, anxiety and, improved quality of life. While one can understand most patients are fooled by the immediate effects of smoking on perceived stress (decreasing cerebral pain from nicotine withdrawal), the fact that too many psychiatric setting remain smokehouses question the quality of care.(3) Similarly, in many experience, as a second line specialist for severe alcohol use disorders, many of patients referred t...
Davis and colleagues must be commended for their concern about cancer outcomes in patients with mental disorders and for acknowledging the poor quality of research.(1) However, their statement “pre-existing mental disorder have a higher odds of advanced stage cancer at diagnosis “ deserve comment.
Firstly, patients with mental disorders, as all vulnerable populations, have poor access to care, considering either quantitatively or qualitatively, even more for specialized care, whatever it could be. Accordingly, a vertical approach only targeting patients with mental disorders would only be a partial and symptomatic solution A root cause analysis is a prerequisite to expect fixing a systemic failure.
Secondly, the term “pre-existing mental disorder” must be questioned as tobacco and alcohol cause both mental health problems and cancer. There is robust and accumulating evidence that cigarette smoking is a causal risk factor for anxiety, depression and, even severe mental illness such as bipolar disorder.(2) Cessation is associated with reduced depression, anxiety and, improved quality of life. While one can understand most patients are fooled by the immediate effects of smoking on perceived stress (decreasing cerebral pain from nicotine withdrawal), the fact that too many psychiatric setting remain smokehouses question the quality of care.(3) Similarly, in many experience, as a second line specialist for severe alcohol use disorders, many of patients referred to me are treated with antidepressants. Antidepressants have modest, if any, useful effects in depressed drinkers but have a potential to aggravate drinking outcomes (pathological intoxication with marked lost control and, occasionally serious violence, even suicide or homicide).(4) This is a real issue in France: a) the use of alcohol, a most depressive agent, is a public health crisis as in England; b) in contrast to England psychotherapy is not reimbursed by the French mandatory healthcare scheme yet despite the success of the Improving Access to Psychological Therapies programme launched in 2008.
Last, even if a pre-existing mental disorder can to be the main concern, as in patients with schizophrenia, no one must overlook that these patients are 3.5 times more likely to die than the general population due to lung cancer, chronic obstructive pulmonary and cardiovascular diseases. For the main psychiatric cause of death, suicide, whose prevention remains a challenge, standardized mortality ratios is 52/100,000 person-years vs 75 for lung cancer.(5) In contrast, adequate treatment for smoking cessation, psychotherapy and nicotine replacement therapy with the belt and brace strategy (patches plus oral forms to suppress occasional craving, at increasing doses) is most effective, whether there are mental health disorders or not.
1 Davis LE, Bogner E, Coburn NG et al. Stage at diagnosis and survival in patients with cancer and a pre-existing mental illness: a meta-analysis. J Epidemiol Community Health 2020;74:84-94.
2 Vermeulen JM, Wootton RE, Treur JL et al. Smoking and the risk for bipolar disorder: evidence from a bidirectional Mendelian randomisation study. Br J Psychiatry. 2019. Online Sep 17. doi: 10.1192/bjp.2019.202.
3 Evins AE, Cather C, Daumit GL. Smoking cessation in people with serious mental illness. Lancet Psychiatry2019;6:563-564.
4 Braillon A. Alcohol Use Disorders and the Barrel of the Danaids. Alcohol Alcohol 2016;51:774.
5 Gatov E, Rosella L, Chiu M, et al. Trends in standardized mortality among individuals with schizophrenia, 1993–2012: a population-based, repeated cross-sectional study. CMAJ 2017;189:E1177–87.
As Prof. Young-Ho Khang points out, numerator-denominator bias may affect the estimation of mortality for the Korean and Japanese populations, because we used a cross-sectional unlinked design.[1] We mentioned the possibility of this bias in our paper, citing a study from Lithuania, which suggests that the mortality of persons with high socioeconomic status may be underestimated as a result of this bias.[2] However, based on a national validation study Prof. Khang suggests that the direction of this bias may work the other way around in the Korean population.[3] Furthermore, because – according to his information – the registration of occupation has changed in South Korea, Prof. Khang also claims that the deterioration of the mortality rates among upper non-manual workers observed in our paper is likely to be an artefact.
While we agree with Prof. Khang that the direction of the numerator-denominator bias may be different in South Korea as compared to Lithuania, we do not agree that the ‘reverse’ manual/non-manual mortality rate ratio that we found in South Korea can be explained by this bias, or that the unfavourable mortality trends among upper non-manual workers that we observed in South Korea can be explained by a change in registering occupation. Our findings prior to 2005 are similar to those of a longitudinal study that followed participants between 1995 and 2008 and reported low mortality among male managers and professional workers in South Korea.[4] Our stu...
As Prof. Young-Ho Khang points out, numerator-denominator bias may affect the estimation of mortality for the Korean and Japanese populations, because we used a cross-sectional unlinked design.[1] We mentioned the possibility of this bias in our paper, citing a study from Lithuania, which suggests that the mortality of persons with high socioeconomic status may be underestimated as a result of this bias.[2] However, based on a national validation study Prof. Khang suggests that the direction of this bias may work the other way around in the Korean population.[3] Furthermore, because – according to his information – the registration of occupation has changed in South Korea, Prof. Khang also claims that the deterioration of the mortality rates among upper non-manual workers observed in our paper is likely to be an artefact.
While we agree with Prof. Khang that the direction of the numerator-denominator bias may be different in South Korea as compared to Lithuania, we do not agree that the ‘reverse’ manual/non-manual mortality rate ratio that we found in South Korea can be explained by this bias, or that the unfavourable mortality trends among upper non-manual workers that we observed in South Korea can be explained by a change in registering occupation. Our findings prior to 2005 are similar to those of a longitudinal study that followed participants between 1995 and 2008 and reported low mortality among male managers and professional workers in South Korea.[4] Our study, however, also finds that after 2010 mortality among upper non-manual workers rapidly increased, causing a reversal of the upper non-manual/manual rate ratio. It is unlikely that this is due to a change in the recording of occupation. According to our information, there was a slight change in the method of recording occupation on Korean death certificates during the 1990s, but there has been no further changes since the year 2000.[5-6] Therefore, we believe that the discrepancy between some Korean studies and our results mentioned by Prof. Khang is due to a difference in study period.
Prof. Khang’s letter implicitly also suggests the possibility that numerator-denominator bias explains our findings on Japan. In contrast to South Korea, there are no longitudinal studies in Japan in which occupational class at baseline can be related to mortality during follow-up. We therefore can only speculate about the magnitude and direction of this bias. However, the method of recording occupation on death certificates or the census has not changed during the study period in Japan, and the deterioration of the mortality rate among upper non-manual workers is therefore unlikely to be an artefact.
Reference
1. Tanaka H, Nusselder WJ, Bopp M, et al. Mortality inequalities by occupational class among men in Japan, South Korea and eight European countries: a national register-based study, 1990–2015. J Epidemiol Community Health 2019;73:750-758. doi:10.1136/jech-2018-211715
2. Shkolnikov VM, Jasilionis D, Andreev EM, et al. Linked versus unlinked estimates of mortality and length of life by education and marital status: evidence from the first record linkage study in Lithuania. Soc Sci Med 2007;64:1392–406. doi:10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.11.014
3. Kim HR, Khang YH. [Reliability of education and occupational class: a comparison of health survey and death certificate data]. J Prev Med Public Health. 2005;38:443-448. (in Korean)
4. Lee H-E, Kim H-R, Chung YK, et al. Mortality rates by occupation in Korea: a nationwide, 13-year follow-up study. Occup Environ Med 2016;73:329–35. doi:10.1136/oemed-2015-103192
5. Vital Statistics Division, Social Statistics Bureau, Statistics Korea (KOSTAT) [Internet]. Available: http://kostat.go.kr/portal/eng/aboutUs/3/2/9/2/index.static (Accessed 23 Sep 2019)
6. Supreme Court of Korea. Regulation on Document Form of Family Related Registration (in Korean) [Internet]. Available: https://glaw.scourt.go.kr/wsjo/gchick/sjo330.do?contId=2258861&q=%EA%B0%... (Accessed 24 Sep 2019)
The authors, Cifuentes MP, Rodriguez-Villamizar LA, Rojas-Botero ML, et al [1], present an article that, owing to a lack of rigor in the creation and application of ethno-racial categories, ends up employing an analysis method that, although intended to allow proving inequalities, ends up disguising or attenuating them.
Raj Bhopal published a seminal article in this journal in 2004 demonstrating the importance of ethnicity and race variables in epidemiology and public health. Bhopal pointed out that, at a minimum, researchers should explain their understanding of the concepts of race or ethnicity and the classification they use, even more so when we know that they need development in terms of geographic specificity, scope, and precision for different contexts [2]. Similar recommendations are made by Janeth Mosquera in her analysis about the use of the ethnic-racial category in the research published by the three most important scientific journals of Public Health in Colombia [3].
The paper does not present a comprehensive and helpful description of the categories that assist the reader in understanding the ethnic-racial composition of the Colombian population and correctly analyze the regularly available data for public health surveillance. The Colombian surveillance system employs the census ethnic-racial categories. Among these Census categories, the "white-mestizo" used by the authors is not defined and thus is not used for public health surveill...
Show MoreFrank’s essay contains several statements about 5G, its relation to the radiation protection science, and related to this, ICNIRP’s guidance and integrity more generally. ICNIRP considers this to be seriously inaccurate and in need of correction for the sake of both scientific accuracy and development of effective public health policy. However, due to journal word limits we must restrict our response to Frank’s misleading claims about ICNIRP’s integrity (for full response see https://www.icnirp.org/en/activities/news/index.html).
Frank’s essay accuses ICNIRP of unmanaged conflict of interests, and uses this accusation to attempt to throw doubt on ICNIRP’s scientific evaluations. However, ICNIRP has a very rigorous procedure to avoid conflicts of interest (https://www.icnirp.org/en/about-icnirp/commission/index.html), and Frank did not provide any evidence in support of his statement - he merely referred to ‘persistent allegations’ from the Swedish epidemiologist Lennart Hardell. For example, Frank repeats claims made by Hardell that “ICNIRP’s membership includes over-representation of vested interests, especially the giant multinational telecommunications firms who are heavily invested in the roll out of 5G systems internationally”, and no supporting evidence was provided by either author. To be clear, there are no industry r...
Show MoreHengartner et al. conducted a meta-analysis on suicide risk with selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) and other new-generation antidepressants in adults (1). Although the pooled relative risks (RRs) of SSRI for suicide risk including suicide and suicide attempt in patients with depression and in patients with all indications did not reach the level of significance, the pooled RR (95% confidence intervals [CIs]) of any new-generation antidepressant for suicide risk in patients with depression and in patients with all indications were 1.29 (1.06-1.57) and 1.45 (1.23-1.70), respectively. The authors presented information on the different suicide risk between SSRI and other new-generation antidepressants , and I present additional information regarding the relationship.
First, Sharma et al. conducted a meta-analysis on the association of SSRI and serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors with suicidality and other mental indicators (2). Although the pooled odds ratios (ORs) of antidepressant treatment for suicidality and aggression did not reach the level of significance in adults, the pooed ORs (95% CIs) of antidepressant treatment for suicidality and aggression were 2.39 (1.31-4.33) and 2.79 (1.62-4.81) in children/adolescents. The suicide risk differed in different generations, and suicide risk estimation should be conducted by stratification with generation and type of anti-depressants.
Second, Hengartner and Plöderl reported that odds ratios (OR...
Show MoreWe read with interest, intrigue and concern the findings reported in this short report. if the findings are validated from larger and multicentric data this may have huge implications in the way we trace and isolate the COVID-19 contacts. Pre symptomatic transmission from index cases 5 days prior to the onset of symptoms is a huge logistical nightmare in terms of containment strategies. This would imply at practical impossibility and futility of these strategy especially in setting of cluster or community transmission. This also highlights the virtues of basic but universal measures like physical distancing, hygiene and use of mask at all times under specific settings.
The question “Why does Russia have such high cardiovascular (CV) mortality rates?”1 can be answered by a pathologist who practiced during the Soviet time.2 Since then, the quality of post mortem examinations has decreased especially during the 1990s: autopsies were sometimes made perfunctorily. The deterioration in anatomic pathology and the health care in general during the 1990s coincided with the increase in the registered CV mortality. A tendency to over-diagnose CV diseases is generally known to exist also for people dying at home and not undergoing autopsy. If a cause of death is not entirely clear, it has been usual to write on a death certificate: “Ischemic heart disease with cardiac insufficiency” or a similar formulation.2 Concerning the relatively high CV mortality in Russia, it should be commented that irregular treatment of hypertension,3 diabetes and other chronic diseases continues to be a problem. Considering the above, the differences between Norwegian and Russian cohorts1 can be better understood. The levels of serum lipids were comparable between Russia and Norway being slightly higher in the latter possibly due to better nutrition. Interestingly, N-terminal pro-b-type natriuretic peptide (NT-proBNP), high-sensitivity cardiac troponin T (hs-cTnT), and high-sensitivity C-reactive protein (hsCRP) were higher in Russia.1 It can be reasonably assumed that average levels of these markers inversely correlate with a nation’s health reflected by the life expecta...
Show MoreWe read with great interest the report from Hamer and colleagues that examined the hypothesis that associations between television (TV) viewing and mortality from heart disease (HD) are due to confounding (1). They employed a negative control approach (2) and report evidence of associations between TV viewing and HD mortality (HR=1.09 [1.06, 1.12] per 1 hr/day increase in TV) and accidental deaths (the negative control outcome; HR=1.06 [0.98, 1.15]) after adjusting for age, sex, smoking, education, and prevalent HD (1)
The positive association between TV and accidental deaths was interpreted as evidence that the TV-HD mortality association was due to confounding. Although key study limitations were noted including a small number of accidental deaths and limited adjustment for confounding, the authors concluded that “observed associations between TV and HD are likely to be driven by confounding”. Although we agree that confounding is a worrisome threat to the internal validity of epidemiologic studies, we believe that the conclusion in the Hamer report is overstated.
A critical additional strategy to understand bias due to confounding, one that was not employed in the current study, is to examine relevant results from published studies conducted in different study populations using different methods. (2) We previously reported results in two studies that examined associations for accidental deaths and HD mortality with TV viewing (3) and leisure-time sitti...
Show MoreDear Editor,
Show MoreThe authors of “Effects of physical activity calorie equivalent food labelling to reduce food selection and consumption: systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled studies” make a strong claim that PACE food labeling can increase consumer consciousness of calorie consumption and therefore caloric reduction, but perhaps this concept deters true understanding of "healthful eating" and may have larger health implications for those with disordered eating.
There is a growing knowledge that that not all calories are created equal. Different foods may not only have different effects on hunger and satiety but also insulin production, gut microbiome interactions, and de novo lipogenesis in the liver (1). While not all consumers need this level of understanding, but without a basic acknowledgement of food’s qualities- like fats, fiber, sugar, ect- the consumer is lead to believe that calories are the most important determinant in what makes food “healthful.” With PACE food labeling, a consumer is led to believe that an ice cream cone and a handful of nuts, both of which could amount to 200 calories, are “equal.” However, in this comparison, only the nuts are possibly advantageous to people with diabetes and cardiovascular disease (2).
Stripping foods down to solely their caloric energy through PACE food labeling could inadvertently foster unhealthy relationships with food. As stated in the article, PACE labeling could be use...
Dear Editor,
Much has been published in the news as of late about the effects of physical activity calorie equivalent (PACE) food labelling in order to reduce the nation’s calorie consumption. These labels aim to identify how many minutes of physical activity are required to burn off the calories in a particular food item. A systematic review and meta-analysis, by researchers at Loughborough University, found that food labelling may reduce the number of calories consumed compared with food that was not labelled or other types of food labelling (1).
This was supported by the UK Royal Society for Public Health which had already advocated for PACE to replace the current labelling system (2). Overall, it found this technique could lead to a reduction of 100 calories per day combined with an increase in physical activity.
Many nutritionists have been quick to criticise, stating that it loses sight of the fact that food goes beyond calories and is fundamental for social aspects of life (3). Additionally, the nutritional content of food might be neglected. For example, it might be easier to “burn off” a chocolate bar than something with much more nutritious such as nut butters or a banana. This could result in people picking the easier but not necessarily the “healthier option.” Digestion is complex and although foods such as nuts and oats might be high in calories, their content results in slower processing and digestion. This allows people to feel fuller fo...
Show MoreDavis and colleagues must be commended for their concern about cancer outcomes in patients with mental disorders and for acknowledging the poor quality of research.(1) However, their statement “pre-existing mental disorder have a higher odds of advanced stage cancer at diagnosis “ deserve comment.
Firstly, patients with mental disorders, as all vulnerable populations, have poor access to care, considering either quantitatively or qualitatively, even more for specialized care, whatever it could be. Accordingly, a vertical approach only targeting patients with mental disorders would only be a partial and symptomatic solution A root cause analysis is a prerequisite to expect fixing a systemic failure.
Secondly, the term “pre-existing mental disorder” must be questioned as tobacco and alcohol cause both mental health problems and cancer. There is robust and accumulating evidence that cigarette smoking is a causal risk factor for anxiety, depression and, even severe mental illness such as bipolar disorder.(2) Cessation is associated with reduced depression, anxiety and, improved quality of life. While one can understand most patients are fooled by the immediate effects of smoking on perceived stress (decreasing cerebral pain from nicotine withdrawal), the fact that too many psychiatric setting remain smokehouses question the quality of care.(3) Similarly, in many experience, as a second line specialist for severe alcohol use disorders, many of patients referred t...
Show MoreAs Prof. Young-Ho Khang points out, numerator-denominator bias may affect the estimation of mortality for the Korean and Japanese populations, because we used a cross-sectional unlinked design.[1] We mentioned the possibility of this bias in our paper, citing a study from Lithuania, which suggests that the mortality of persons with high socioeconomic status may be underestimated as a result of this bias.[2] However, based on a national validation study Prof. Khang suggests that the direction of this bias may work the other way around in the Korean population.[3] Furthermore, because – according to his information – the registration of occupation has changed in South Korea, Prof. Khang also claims that the deterioration of the mortality rates among upper non-manual workers observed in our paper is likely to be an artefact.
Show MoreWhile we agree with Prof. Khang that the direction of the numerator-denominator bias may be different in South Korea as compared to Lithuania, we do not agree that the ‘reverse’ manual/non-manual mortality rate ratio that we found in South Korea can be explained by this bias, or that the unfavourable mortality trends among upper non-manual workers that we observed in South Korea can be explained by a change in registering occupation. Our findings prior to 2005 are similar to those of a longitudinal study that followed participants between 1995 and 2008 and reported low mortality among male managers and professional workers in South Korea.[4] Our stu...
Pages