Article Text

other Versions

Download PDFPDF

Association of serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D3 and D2 with academic performance in childhood: findings from a prospective birth cohort
  1. Anna-Maija Tolppanen1,
  2. Adrian Sayers2,
  3. William D Fraser3,
  4. Debbie A Lawlor1
  1. 1MRC Centre for Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
  2. 2School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Bristol, UK
  3. 3Norwich Medical School, University of East Anglia, Norwich, UK
  1. Correspondence to Dr Anna-Maija Tolppanen, MRC Centre for Causal Analysis in Translational Epidemiology, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, 15-23 Oakfield Grove, Clifton, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK; anna-maija.tolppanen{at}uef.fi Professor Debbie A Lawlor, MRC Centre for Causal Analysis in Translational Epidemiology, School of Social and Community Medicine, University of Bristol, Oakfield House, 15-23 Oakfield Grove, Clifton, Bristol BS8 2BN, UK; d.a.lawlor{at}bristol.ac.uk

Abstract

Background Higher total serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) concentrations have been associated with better cognitive function mainly in cross-sectional studies in adults. It is unknown if the associations of different forms of 25(OH)D (25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2) are similar.

Methods Prospective cohort study (n=3171) with serum 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2 concentrations measured at mean age of 9.8 years and academic performance at age 13–14 years (total scores in English, mathematics and science) and 15–16 years (performance in General Certificates of Education examinations).

Results Serum 25(OH)D3 concentrations were not associated with any educational outcomes. Higher 25(OH)D2 concentrations were associated with worse performance in English at age 13–14 years (adjusted SD change per doubling in 25(OH)D2 (95% CI) −0.05 (−0.08 to −0.01)) and with worse academic performance at age 15–16 years (adjusted OR for obtaining ≥5 A*–C grades (95% CI) 0.91 (0.82 to 1.00)).

Conclusion The null findings with 25(OH)D3 are in line with two previous cross-sectional studies in children. It is possible that the positive association of 25(OH)D with cognitive function seen in adults does not emerge until later in life or that the results from previous cross-sectional adult studies are due to reverse causality. The unexpected inverse association of 25(OH)D2 with academic performance requires replication in further studies. Taken together, our findings do not support suggestions that children should have controlled exposure to sunlight, or vitamin D supplements, in order to increase academic performance.

  • 25-Hydroxyvitamin D
  • child
  • adolescent
  • cognitive function
  • ALSPAC
  • epidemiology
  • cognition
  • child health
  • asthma
  • atopy

This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution Non-commercial License, which permits use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited, the use is non commercial and is otherwise in compliance with the license. See: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/ and http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/2.0/legalcode.

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Introduction

Cognitive function in childhood and educational attainment are associated with adverse health outcomes in adulthood.1–9 These associations are approximately linear across the full distribution of cognitive function/education, rather than being driven by adverse health in those with severe learning difficulties.9 Understanding the determinants of childhood cognitive function and educational attainment are important for understanding the mechanisms linking this to later health and for developing means to improve educational attainment and later health and well-being.

Higher serum concentrations of total 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) have been associated with better cognitive function in some,10–15 though not all,16 17 epidemiological studies of adults. To our knowledge, only two studies have examined this association in children; both these cross-sectional studies used data from the Third US National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey (NHANES III) and found no association between total 25(OH)D and childhood cognitive function.17 18

Circulatory 25(OH)D consists of 25(OH)D3 (synthesised from vitamin D3 obtained mainly from ultraviolet B (UVB) induced synthesis in skin and to a lesser extent from animal food sources) and 25(OH)D2 (synthesised from vitamin D2 obtained from plant/fungal sources). There is evidence that vitamin D3 supplementation is more potent in raising and maintaining 25(OH)D concentrations than supplementation with vitamin D2.19 It is unknown if these two forms of 25(OH)D have similar associations with cognitive function.

The aim of this study was to examine the prospective association between variation in serum 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2 with academic performance in children and to compare if these associations were similar with both forms of 25(OH)D. Because vitamin D, together with parathyroid hormone (PTH), regulates calcium and phosphate homoeostasis,20 21 we investigated whether these analytes were related to academic performance and whether the association of 25(OH)D3 and or 25(OH)D2 was independent of PTH, calcium or phosphate.

Methods

Population

The Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children (ALSPAC) is a population-based birth cohort from South West England. The cohort consisted of 14 062 live births from 14 541 enrolled pregnant women who were expected to give birth between 1 April 1991 and 31 December 1992.22 From age 7, all children were invited for an annual assessment of physical and psychological development. Parents gave informed consent at enrolment, and ethical approval was obtained from the ALSPAC Law and Ethics Research Committee and local research ethics committee. Permission to use education data was sought from the accompanying adult at the 7-year clinic.

Single and twin births were included in this study. Those with any special education needs at the first four academic performance assessments (key stage (KS) assessments) were excluded. Figure 1 shows the flow through the cohort of participants and how the number included in the analyses presented here was derived. In total, 3171 participants with complete data on both exposures, all outcomes and confounders were included in our analyses.

Figure 1

Flow of participants.

Exposures and related blood-based covariables

Serum 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2, PTH, phosphate and calcium were assayed on non-fasting blood samples collected at mean age of 9.9 years for the majority of participants. If no samples were available from the 9.9 years assessment, samples from mean age of 11.8 years or, second, the 7.6 years assessment were used. The mean age at sample collection in the whole study sample was 9.8 years (SD: 1.1). Following collection, samples were immediately spun, frozen and stored at −80°C. Assays were performed after a maximum of 12 years in storage, with no previous freeze-thaw cycles. 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2 and deuterated internal standard were measured with high performance liquid chromatography tandem mass spectrometer using multiple reaction mode with following mass:charge ratio transitions: 413.2>395.3, 401.1>383.3 and 407.5>107.2 for 25(OH)D2, 25(OH)D3 and hexa-deuterated (OH)D3, respectively. Interassay coefficients of variation for the assay were <10% across a working range of 2.5–624 nmol/l for both 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2.

Total serum calcium, phosphate and albumin concentrations were measured by standard laboratory methods on Roche Modular analysers (Roche Diagnostics Ltd, West Sussex, UK). Serum calcium was adjusted for albumin using a normogram of calcium and albumin distributions of the samples analysed in the clinical chemistry laboratory where the measurements were performed. Albumin-adjusted calcium concentrations were used in all statistical analyses. Intact parathyroid hormone (iPTH(1–84)) was measured by electrochemiluminescent immunoassay on an Elecsys 2010 immunoanalyzer (Roche, Lewes, UK). Interassay coefficient of variation was <6% from 2 to 50 pmol/l. The assay sensitivity (replicates of the zero standard) was 1 pmol/l.

Outcomes

KS3 and KS4 results were obtained by linkage to National Pupil Database, a central repository for pupil-level annual school census, using unique pupil numbers as identifiers. KS3 tests are performed at 13–14 years (school year 9) when performance in English, maths and science is assessed. Total point scores in each subject were used in the analyses. KS4 is performed at 15–16 years when pupils take their General Certificates of Education in a range of academic and applied subjects. General Certificates of Education are graded from A* to G, with A* indicating the highest performance. We examined associations with three outcomes from KS4 assessments: (1) those achieving five or more A*–C grades, including maths and English (this variable is provided in the National Pupil Data set and is the level of achievement generally required to go on to advance level study and ultimately university; many semi-skilled jobs that do not require a university degree also consider this an important achievement); (2) those achieving nine or more A*–A (an indicator of the highest performers) and (3) those achieving no A*–C grades (an indicator of poor performance).

Confounders

Data on ethnicity, maternal and paternal education and occupational social class were obtained from parent-completed questionnaires. Data on subsidised school meals were obtained from the National Pupil Database. Data on time spent outdoors and protection from UVB exposure (use of sunblock, covering clothing or hat and avoidance of midday sun) were obtained from parent-completed questionnaires at mean age of 8.5 years. Body mass index (BMI) was calculated from height and weight measured at the same time as blood samples were taken. IQ was assessed by the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-IIIUK) at the 8–9-year assessment.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted with Stata V.11.0 (Stata Corp LP). To adjust for seasonal variability, 25(OH)D3 was modelled according to date of blood sampling using linear regression with trigonometric sine and cosine functions. 25(OH)D3 was loge transformed to reduce heteroscedasticity. The residual from the sine–cosine date of sampling regression model was then used as the primary 25(OH)D3 exposure variable in subsequent regression analyses. To include all participants on whom a 25(OH)D2 was assayed, those with a value below the detectable limit of the assay (1.25 nmol/l, n=1147, 36%) were given a value of 1.25 nmol/l and indicated using a binary covariable in all regression models.

Age and gender SD (z) scores for serum 25(OH)D2 and 25(OH)D3, PTH, calcium and phosphate were generated using the internal cohort data with age in 1 month categories. The association of potential confounders with KS3 outcomes and with exposures was assessed with linear regression, and the association of confounders with KS4 outcomes was assessed with logistic regression.

The associations of 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2 with KS3 and KS4 results were assessed with non-parametric bootstrap procedure in conjunction with multilevel mixed-effects linear regression (for KS3 outcomes) or random-effects logit models (for KS4 outcomes) based on 10 000 replications. The multilevel mixed-effects linear regression and random-effects logit models were used to take into account the school-level variance in the KS results, and the bootstrapping procedure enabled us to statistically compare associations of 25(OH)D2 to those of 25(OH)D3. The difference between the association of 25(OH)D2 and 25(OH)D3 was calculated from the bootstrap replicate distribution. β-Estimates and SEs were empirically calculated from the mean and SD of the bootstrap distribution, respectively. All p values were calculated using bootstrap means and SEs and compared to a z-distribution. To numerically compare the associations of two forms of 25(OH)D, β-coefficients from the regression models were multiplied by loge(2). These are then interpreted as mean differences or ORs per doubling of exposure. For the main analyses presented here, we compared the associations of season-adjusted 25(OH)D3 to those of 25(OH)D2. The associations of unadjusted (for season) 25(OH)D3 and total 25(OH)D (25(OH)D3, with no seasonal adjustment, plus 25(OH)D2) with all outcomes are shown in the supplementary material.

Results

The mean (SD) seasonally adjusted serum 25(OH)D3, phosphate and albumin-adjusted calcium concentration were 62.1 (0.8) nmol/l, 1.53 (0.16) mmol/l and 2.38 (0.12) mmol/l, respectively. The median (IQR) for serum 25(OH)D2 and PTH were 3.2 (1.3–6.7) nmol/l and 4.4 (3.3–5.8) pmol/l, respectively. Mean (SD) score in KS3 English, maths and science examinations were 54 (15), 89 (20) and 102 (22), respectively.

Supplementary table 1 shows the distribution of outcomes and confounders in participants with data on outcomes who were excluded due to missing data in exposures or confounders and in complete cases. Complete cases were more likely to perform better in both KS examinations, have higher IQ and lower BMI, to be of white ethnicity, less meticulous about protection from UVB and from higher socioeconomic backgrounds than those with missing data on exposures or confounders.

Supplementary table 2 shows the associations of potential confounders with 25(OH)D2, 25(OH)D3, PTH, calcium and phosphate. Those with non-white ethnicity had higher PTH and lower 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2 concentrations. BMI was negatively associated with serum 25(OH)D3 and positively with PTH concentrations. Higher socioeconomic position was associated with higher concentrations of 25(OH)D3 and phosphate and lower concentrations of 25(OH)D2 and calcium. Children who were protected more from UVB exposure had lower 25(OH)D3, phosphate and PTH concentrations and higher calcium concentrations. Time spent outdoors during summer was positively associated with 25(OH)D3 concentrations. Pupils who were eligible for free school meals had lower 25(OH)D3 and higher 25(OH)D2 and PTH concentrations.

Supplementary table 3 shows the associations of potential confounders with KS3 scores. Non-white ethnicity, higher BMI, lower socioeconomic position, being more meticulous about avoiding UVB exposure and spending less time outdoors were all associated with lower examination scores in at least one of English, maths or science. Supplementary table 4 summarises the associations of potential confounders and academic performance in KS4. In general, these associations were similar to those found with KS3 scores.

Table 1 shows the prospective associations of serum 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2, calcium, phosphate and PTH concentrations with KS3 scores. 25(OH)D3 concentrations were not associated with any KS3 outcomes. Higher 25(OH)D2 concentrations were associated with worse performance in English in the unadjusted model (model 1) and after adjusting for confounders including ethnicity, socioeconomic position, protection from UVB (model 2) and for other analytes, including 25(OH)D3 (model 3). 25(OH)D2 was not associated with performance in maths or science. The associations of 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2 with English were different from each other (p=0.01). None of PTH, calcium or phosphate was associated with KS3 outcomes in confounder-adjusted models.

Table 1

Prospective association of 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2, phosphate, calcium and PTH concentrations (assessed at mean age of 9.8 years with key stage 3 scores (assessed at age 13–14 years) (n=3171)

Table 2 shows the prospective association between exposures and KS4 outcomes. These were essentially consistent with findings seen for KS3 outcomes. 25(OH)D3 was not associated with any of the KS4 outcomes, and higher serum 25(OH)D2 concentrations were associated with worse academic performance (less likely to obtain ≥5 A*–C grades or ≥9 A*–A grades) in the unadjusted model (model 1). These inverse associations attenuated slightly with adjustment for potential confounders (model 2), with additional adjustment for other analytes, including 25(OH)D3 not changing results further (model 3). There was statistical evidence that the associations of 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2 with obtaining ≥5 A*–C grades differed from each other (p=0.02). PTH, calcium and phosphate concentrations were not associated with any KS4 outcomes, with the exception of phosphate having a modest positive association with obtaining ≥5 A*–C grades. The associations of unadjusted (for season) 25(OH)D3 and total 25(OH)D concentrations with educational outcomes were similar to those of season-adjusted 25(OH)D3 shown here (supplementary tables 5 and 6).

Table 2

Prospective association of 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2, phosphate, calcium and PTH concentrations (assessed at mean age of 9.8 years) with key stage 4 educational attainment (age 15–16 years) (n=3171)

Supplementary tables 7 and 8 show the results of sensitivity analyses restricted to participants with data on IQ at age 8.5 years and exposure measurements at age 9.9 or 11.8 years. Adjustment for IQ at age 8.5 years, in addition to other confounders, did not alter the inverse associations of 25(OH)D2 with outcomes.

Discussion

Serum 25(OH)D3 concentrations, at age 7.6–11.8 years, were not associated with academic performance assessed at age 13–14 or 15–16 years in our prospective study. Somewhat surprisingly serum concentrations of 25(OH)D2 were negatively associated with academic achievements. This association was consistent with the associations of indicators of lower socioeconomic position (eg, eligibility for free school meals, lower parental education and socioeconomic position) with lower educational attainment and higher 25(OH)D2 concentrations. It is therefore possible that it represents residual confounding. There was little evidence that PTH, calcium or phosphate were associated with educational attainment. These findings build on two previous cross-sectional studies in children using NHANES data that reported no associations of total 25(OH)D or calcium with cognitive function.17 18 Since 25(OH)D3 is the major contributor to total 25(OH)D,23 our findings of no association of 25(OH)D3 or calcium with academic performance are consistent with these studies. This is illustrated in our cohort by the lack of association of total 25 (OH)D (supplementary table 6) with any of the educational outcomes.

Most cross-sectional10 12–15 and one of the two prospective11 studies in adults have shown a positive association between total 25(OH)D concentrations and cognitive function, though a recent second prospective study found no association between total 25(OH)D and cognitive decline.16 The difference between the overall body of evidence in adults (suggesting better cognitive function in those with higher vitamin D) compared with children (no association) could be because the key effects of 25(OH)D only emerge in adults either because it is related more to cognitive decline than development or because it is a lifetime cumulative amount of vitamin D that is important with respect to cognitive function. Alternatively, it could be that studies of adults (which are mostly cross-sectional) are more likely to be affected by reverse causality than those in childhood (with adults with poorer cognitive function spending less time outdoors and hence having lower 25(OH)D concentrations).

Experimental studies suggest that vitamin D and its metabolites are important for neuronal function.24–26 The findings from these studies suggest that vitamin D may be linked to cognition by different pathways, including mediation of inflammatory response, regulation of genes that mediate neuronal density, synaptic plasticity, neurotransmitter synthesis and survival and differentiation of neurons, as well as direct functions as a neurotrophic and neuroprotective factor.27 Although animal studies have shown that vitamin D-deficient animals often display behavioural abnormalities and different learning patterns, there are some inconsistencies in these results (reviewed in McCann and Ames27 and Eyles et al28). The majority of the animal studies have used vitamin D receptor knockout models or restricted the UV irradiation/dietary intake of vitamin D3, which would provide more information on the effects of vitamin D3 and its metabolites. Thus, it is difficult to suggest a biological mechanism, which could explain the negative association between 25(OH)D2 and academic performance based on these possible pathways. One possibility is that this is a chance finding given the number of associations that have been examined in this paper and the lack of any previous studies (that we are aware of) that have examined the associations of 25(OH)D2 specifically with cognitive or educational outcomes. It is also possible that despite extensive adjustment for potential confounders, the association is explained by residual confounding and different confounder structures between the two forms of 25(OH)D. The main source of 25(OH)D2 are diet and supplements, whereas that of 25(OH)D3 is UVB exposure. Dietary behaviours may be more likely to be affected by multiple confounders that could generate the 25(OH)D2 association than is UVB exposure. However, for this to be the case, we would need to think of a (or several) confounders that are related to higher levels of consumption of food that is a source of 25(OH)D2 (mostly supplements) and lower educational attainment.

Strengths and weaknesses

In addition to examining prospective associations, the strengths of the present study include large sample size, ability to adjust for multiple confounders and the measurement methodology for 25(OH)D concentrations. With one exception,16 previous studies of associations in adults and children have used immunoassays, which may underestimate the amount of 25(OH)D2,29 30 or 25(OH)D in general,31 32 and do not enable the separate measurements of 25(OH)D2 and 25(OH)D3. It is valuable to examine the associations of these two forms of vitamin D with outcomes because supplements are available in both forms, and until recently vitamin D supplements (as opposed to multivitamin supplements) have largely been 25(OH)D2. There is debate about whether the two forms have equivalent potency and whether vitamin D2 supplements are valuable means of increasing total 25(OH)D in humans.19 33 Thus, it is important to know whether the two forms of 25(OH)D do have similar associations with outcomes.

Similar to other prospective cohort studies, there was loss to follow-up and those who have attended follow-up clinics tend to be from higher socioeconomic groups.22 Complete cases were more likely to perform better in KS examinations and be from higher socioeconomic backgrounds than children who were excluded from analyses due to missing data. This may have some implications for the generalisability of the results as children with lower educational attainment were under-represented in the complete case analyses. However, it is unlikely that the associations between vitamin D metabolites and educational attainment would be different in those with missing data. The prevalence of low 25(OH)D concentrations was similar to previous studies (27% of our study population had total 25(OH)D concentration below 20 ng/ml) so our results are likely to be generalisable to most Northern hemisphere populations with similar vitamin D concentrations but may not generalise to those with very different dietary intakes or UVB exposure.

We used academic performance instead of standardised tests of cognitive function, which might complicate the comparison with other studies using IQ tests. Academic performance correlates with IQ test scores (r∼0.5),34 and the correlation coefficients between the full IQ score in Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children at the age of 8.5 years and KS2 and KS3 scores in ALSPAC are similar to these published associations (based on cross-sectional data), ranging between 0.29 and 0.66. Lastly, examining associations with actual educational performance has the advantage that this is a more proximal predictor of future health and well-being and mediates many of the associations of childhood cognition with later adult health outcomes.5–7 We only used data from a single measurement of 25(OH)D3, 25(OH)D2, phosphate, calcium and PTH concentrations, which may be inadequate,35 although a single measurement may be a useful biomarker of season-specific vitamin D status over a longer time,36 and serum calcium concentrations are normally maintained at relatively narrow limits within individuals.37

In conclusion, despite biological evidence that variation in vitamin D and associated biomarkers might influence brain development and hence cognitive function,10 11 28 38 our prospective study suggests that neither 25(OH)D3 nor total 25(OH)D is an important determinant of academic performance in children and that 25(OH)D2 is actually inversely associated with performance. The increasing number of association studies suggesting that low levels of vitamin D are related to a wide range of health outcomes, including cognitive function, have resulted in calls for changes to public health guidance regarding extreme protection against UV exposure. However, our results suggest that protection of children from UVB exposure, which has been associated with low levels of vitamin D, but which protects against skin damage and skin cancer, is unlikely to have any detrimental effect on academic achievement.

What is already known on this subject

  • Higher total serum 25-hydroxyvitamin D (25(OH)D) concentrations have been associated with better cognitive function mainly in cross-sectional studies of adults. There are few prospective studies, and none of these are in children.

  • 25(OH)D is present in two forms: 25(OH)D3 and 25(OH)D2, but it is unknown if the associations of these two forms with cognitive performance are similar.

What this study adds

  • Our prospective study suggests that 25(OH)D3 is not an important determinant of academic performance in children and that 25(OH)D2 is inversely associated with performance, though this may be a chance finding or due to residual confounding.

  • Protection of children from ultraviolet B exposure, which has been associated with low concentrations of vitamin D, but which protects against skin damage and skin cancer, is unlikely to have any detrimental effect on academic achievement.

Acknowledgments

We are extremely grateful to all the families who took part in this study, the midwives for their help in recruiting them and the whole ALSPAC team, which includes interviewers, computer and laboratory technicians, clerical workers, research scientists, volunteers, managers, receptionists and nurses.

References

Supplementary materials

  • Supplementary Data

    This web only file has been produced by the BMJ Publishing Group from an electronic file supplied by the author(s) and has not been edited for content.

    Files in this Data Supplement:

Footnotes

  • Funding Work on this study is funded by an UK Medical Research Council (MRC) Grant G0701603, which also pays A-MT's salary. Salary support for AS is provided by Wellcome Trust grant ref 079960. MRC, the Wellcome Trust and the University of Bristol provide core funding support for ALSPAC. The MRC and the University of Bristol provide core funding for the MRC Centre of Causal Analyses in Translational Epidemiology (Grant G0600705). The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and not necessarily those of any funding body or others whose support is acknowledged. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the manuscript.

  • Competing interests None.

  • Ethics approval ALSPAC Law and Ethics Research Committee and local research ethics committee.

  • Provenance and peer review Not commissioned; externally peer reviewed.