Article Text

Download PDFPDF

Relatively poor, absolutely ill? A study of regional income inequality in Russia and its possible health consequences
Free
  1. Per Carlson
  1. Correspondence to:
 Dr P Carlson
 Stockholm Centre on Health of Societies in Transition, University College of South Stockholm, S-14189 Huddinge, Sweden; per.carlsonsh.se

Abstract

Study objective: To investigate whether the income distribution in a Russian region has a “contextual” effect on individuals’ self rated health, and whether the regional income distributions are related to regional health differences.

Methods: The Russia longitudinal monitoring survey (RLMS) is a survey (n = 7696) that is representative of the Russian population. With multilevel regressions both individual as well as contextual effects on self rated health were estimated.

Main results: The effect of income inequality is not negative on men’s self rated health as long as the level of inequality is not very great. When inequality levels are high, however, there is a tendency for men’s health to be negatively affected. Regional health differences among men are in part explained by regional income differences. On the other hand, women do not seem to be affected in the same way, and individual characteristics like age and educational level seem to be more important.

Conclusions: It seems that a rise in income inequality has no negative effect on men’s self rated health as long as the level of inequality is not very great. On the other hand, when inequality levels are higher a rise tends to affect men’s health negatively. A curvilinear relation between self rated health and income distribution is an interesting hypothesis. It could help to explain the confusing results that arise when you look at countries with a high degree of income inequality (USA) and those with lower income inequality (for example, Japan and New Zealand).

  • income inequality
  • self rated health
  • regions
  • Russia

Statistics from Altmetric.com

Request Permissions

If you wish to reuse any or all of this article please use the link below which will take you to the Copyright Clearance Center’s RightsLink service. You will be able to get a quick price and instant permission to reuse the content in many different ways.

Since the fall of the communist regime in the early 1990s, and with the introduction of “shock therapy” in January 1992, Russia has experienced dramatic economic and social changes. In response to these, Russian public health deteriorated with an increasing mortality and a falling life expectancy.1 Between 1994 and 1998 the life expectancy recovered somewhat, but only to start falling again in 1999 and 2000.2

The economic changes have been difficult for many Russians. Rising unemployment rates and large groups of people falling into poverty can illustrate this. The income distribution has widened substantially, but only a very small group of Russians have benefited economically from the recent changes. A good example of this is that about 80% of the citizens in the city of Taganrog (Rostovskaya Oblast), judged their financial situation to have worsened between 1988 and 1998.3

Moreover, there is good reason to believe that different parts of Russia have been affected differently economically, socially, and in health, with some parts of the country managing the transition better than others. Shkolnikov and Cornia4 found a greater deterioration in life expectancy in the north and northwest regions between 1989 and 1994. Both the Moscow and St Petersburg areas experienced the biggest deteriorations during this period and therefore, they concluded, the decreases cannot be attributable to the economic crisis as these two areas performed relatively better than other areas during this period. Walberg et al5 concluded that the decline in Russian life expectancy cannot be attributed to impoverishment only. Regions that have experienced the most dramatic transition, with, for example, increasing income inequalities as one result, have lost their social cohesion and therefore experienced a rise in mortality. The economic and social changes during the early 1990s, created a psychosocial stress among people that led to harmful behaviours such as increased heavy alcohol consumption or increased criminality.

Kennedy et al6 found an association between indicators of social capital and life expectancy in the Russian regions. Regions with higher mistrust in government, higher criminality, worse working relations, and poorer engagement in politics also had a lower life expectancy. Therefore, they suggested “… that indicators of social capital could account for an important portion of the cross-sectional variation in mortality and life expectancy across the regions of the country.” (page 2037). Moreover, the relation between social capital and mortality persisted when they controlled for per capita income and poverty.

Thus, it seems that poverty in itself cannot explain regional variations in mortality or life expectancy, but rather social and social-psychological factors associated with the economic and political changes.

There has been a lively discussion in the medical and social sciences about whether (and how) income inequalities can affect the health of the individual.7–9 Wilkinson7 suggested that the distribution of incomes in a society may affect people’s health, at least in economically developed societies. It is hypothesised that wide differences in income will negatively affect social cohesion and feelings of trust and solidarity, which in turn will affect all members of the society, but in particular those in a disadvantaged social position. These will feel disadvantaged compared with others higher up. Lynch et al9 are sceptical about this “psychosocial” interpretation and argue that “Interpretation of links between income inequality and health must begin with the structural causes of inequalities, and not just focus on perceptions of that inequality.” (page1200) Income inequality is a consequence of several structural processes and these also affect the individual’s possibilities of getting, for example, education, occupation, food, housing, health services, etc, which all are related to health. In other words, to understand the relation between income inequality and health, you should focus on those processes that create these inequalities. It is reasonable to believe that these structural processes have both psychosocial as well as material effects on health.10 However, the question is how to separate them.

It has also been argued that attention should be directed away from income inequality. Deaton11 argues that income inequality in itself does not have any causal effect on population health and therefore any relation found can be explained by some other factors, for example, poverty. Instead, he argues, focus should be on the health promoting role of income.

Several studies have examined the relation between income inequality and public health in various countries and the results are mixed. Kennedy et al8 found from the USA that people living in states with the greatest inequality were more likely to report poor health than others. On the other hand, evidence from New Zealand (mortality) and Japan (self rated health) show no, or only a very weak, association.12,13 A Canadian study showed some evidence that higher income inequality was related to better health, but the most important factor, however, seemed to be household income.14 Studies on this topic conducted in Russia or other eastern European countries are more sparse. Bobak et al15 found no relation between income inequality and self rated health in seven post-communist countries. Instead, individual factors such as education and material deprivation were most strongly related to self rated health.

The aim of the study is (1) to investigate whether in the regions of Russia income distribution has a “contextual” effect on people’s self rated health, and (2) whether regional income distributions are related to regional differences in health.

DATA AND METHODS

The Russia longitudinal monitoring survey (RLMS) is a survey that is representative of the Russian population and has been carried out at repeated intervals since 1992. It is obviously very difficult to obtain a sample that is representative of the entire Russian population. In brief, the selection procedure entailed the following three steps. (1) In accordance with certain criteria (including degree of urbanisation) a number of regions were selected that together would represent the vast country’s enormous heterogeneity. (2) A number of electoral districts were chosen at random. (3) A register of households in each electoral district was used to select a total of some 7000 households. The response rate was around 90% for the earliest surveys and about 80% for the later ones. In this paper, the 1998 survey is analysed (a detailed explanation of the study design and selection strategy is available on the RLMS web site http://www.cpc.unc.edu/rlms). It contains 7696 interviews (3306 men and 4390 women).

Thus, RLMS is designed to obtain a national representative sample; results at the regional level may therefore not be representative. Thus, to control for statistical problems related to the sample design and to separate the “contextual effects” (that is, the effects of group level properties on the individuals) from individual ones, a multilevel technique with random intercepts was used, whereby you can control for some statistical problems (for example, clustering) and thereafter estimate both individual and contextual effects.16–18 The statistical software used was MLwiN 1.10. The multilevel model includes five levels, but only the regional level and the individual level will be commented on here. The other three levels—that is, family, census district, and population centre—are included only to control for possible design problems related the sampling procedure.

The logic of the regression models look like this;

Embedded Image

β0 is the intercept, and is allowed to vary on five levels: i (individual), j (family), k (census district), l (population centre), and m (primary sampling unit, i.e. region).

β1 represents the effects of the independent variables (here illustrated by x1ijklm) at individual level.

β2 represents the effects of the independent variables (here illustrated by x2jklm) at family level.

β3 represents the effects of the independent variables (here illustrated by x3m) at regional level.

Accordingly, the models allow for random intercepts and fixed effects, which means that the intercepts are allowed to vary between, for example, regions, but the slopes (β) are assumed to be equal.19

Dependent variable

Self rated health is measured on a five point scale indicating whether the respondent rates their health as “very good” (5), “good” (4), “average” (3), “poor” (2) or “very poor” (1).

Independent variables

The household’s reported total income was used to estimate the individual’s economic situation, the mean income level of each region, and the regional income distribution. To control for family composition, the household’s total income was divided by the number of household members. However, the household members were weighted differently. The first adult person in the household had a weight of 1.0, while all others received a weight of 0.8. The most appropriate equivalence scale has been discussed earlier,20 and the choice made here was based on a study from another post-Soviet economy—that is, Estonia.21 The Gini coefficient was used to measure regional income inequality and was calculated as follows;

Embedded Image

N represents the number of households, μ is the mean household income, yi symbolises household “I”, and yj symbolises household “j”. As we do not have any expressed hypotheses about how the relation between income inequality and health may look like, linear or non-linear, we tested several models. A non-linear model, where the Gini coefficient was squared, fitted the data best and was therefore finally kept.

Both at individual and regional level the natural logarithm of household income is used, as it is assumed that the possible effects on health will stagnate at higher income levels, both at individual and regional level.

Background variables

Several background variables are controlled for in the analyses (models 2–4). These are; age (in years), marital status (married or not married), type of settlement ((1) urban, (2) urban-rural, (3) rural) and education ((1) graduate school, (2) university, academy, (3) technical, medical, music, pedagogical, art school, (4) vocational technical school with a secondary education, (5) vocational school without a secondary education, (6) professional courses).

RESULTS

Comparisons and interpretations from table 1 must be carried out with caution, as the results may not be representative for each region. However, we can see significant “regional” variations in self rated health as well as in income and income distribution. One region (Kabardino-Balkarija, Zolskij Rajon) was excluded from further analyses, because of its great deviation from the other regions and it was therefore considered an outlier (see fig 1).

Table 1

 Regional distributions of self rated health, household income, and household income distribution in Russia 1998

Figure 1

 Regional income inequality and mean self rated health in 38 Russian regions.

In tables 2 and 3, men and women were analysed separately.

Table 2

 Self rated health regressed on independent variables (random intercept models): men

Table 3

 Self rated health regressed on independent variables (random intercept models): women

From model 1 (tables 2 and 3) we can see that the average self rated health for men is 3.19, and for women 2.87 (see intercept). As expected, men rate their health better than women. There also seems to be, both for men and women, a significant, but not particularly large, regional proportion, 2.28% and 1.73%, men and women respectively, of the total variation. In model 2 several background variables were included. Age, marital status (for men), and education but not type of settlement had a significant effect on self rated health. In other words, older people, unmarried men (compared with married men), and the lower educated (compared with those with the highest education) tend to rate their health as worse. The regional variation (that is, variance explained by region) seemed to increase somewhat among men (+8.3%, that is, from 2.28% to 2.47%) but decreased considerably among women (−42%). In model 3, the household’s total income was introduced to the model together with the mean household income of the region. None of these economic measures seemed to relate to the individual’s perceived health. The regional variation remained the same, both for men and women.

Finally in model 4, regional income distribution (Gini) was included. For men (table 2), the squared Gini coefficient was significantly related to self rated health, showing a curve-linear relation. Thus, at “lower” levels of inequality an increase seems to slightly improve self rated health, but at “higher” levels an increase seems to have a negative effect on self rated health. However, the effect does not seem to be very strong. Among women there was a similar, but weak and non-significant, curve-linear relation (table 3). For a graphical presentation, see figure 2.

Figure 2

 Regional income inequality and predicted self rated health in Russia.

It is equally interesting to look at changes in the regional variation. Among men, variance explained by region changed from 2.48% (model 3) to 1.36% (model 4), which is a 45% reduction. However, there was no change at all for women.

DISCUSSION

May biases in the analysed data have affected the results? It should be noted that the data are not necessarily representative at the regional level. However, this is not a particular problem because the aim of the study was to analyse the possible relation between income inequality and self rated health, and not to present representative estimations of levels of household income, self rated health, etc. Moreover, the used multilevel design is intended to control for possible statistical problems, for example, clustering. Another problem is the level of aggregation of income inequality. In this study the primary sampling units were used because this is as close as we can get to where the federal political and economic structures are formed. At the same time it is a level that forms a significant cultural identity and to where people can relate. Of course it would be possible to analyse inequalities on other levels, for example cities, neighbourhoods, families, etc, to where people can relate. However, it can still be argued that the chosen level of aggregation here is meaningful, both from the individual perspective and from a macro structural one.

Self rated health was analysed as a continuous variable, but as it is unclear whether this variable forms a continuum or not,22 an additional multilevel logistic regression was performed. However, the results were very similar and the less complex models were kept.

It seems that a rise in income inequality has no negative effect on men’s self rated health as long as the level of inequality is not very high. On the other hand, at higher levels of inequality, a rise tends to affect men’s health negatively. Women do not seem to be affected in the same way. Individual characteristics like age and educational level seem more important. A curvilinear relation between self rated health and income distribution is an interesting hypothesis. It could help to explain the confusing results that arise when you look at countries with a high degree of income inequality (USA) and those with lower income inequality (for example, Japan and New Zealand).

Key points

  • Regional income inequality in Russia seems to affect male self rated health to some extent, even though the effect cannot be seen as very strong.

  • The effect that income inequality has on men’s self rated health is not attributable to their own economic situation or that of the region in which they live.

  • For women, it seems that other factors (age, marital status, type of settlement, and education) are more important.

The regional variation in self rated health cannot be considered large in relation to the total variation and this is, of course, not very surprising. It is more interesting to look at the changes in regional variation between the models. Among women, there was only a change when individual characteristics (age, marital status, type of settlement, and education) were included. Perhaps these factors are more important in explaining female regional variations in self rated health. For men however, the regional variation in self rated health declined when income inequality was taken into account. Moreover, we can conclude that the effect that income inequality has on men’s self rated health is not attributable to their own economic situation or that of the region in which they live.

Thus, regional income inequality in Russia seems to affect male self rated health to some extent, even though the effect cannot be seen as very strong. For women, it seems that other factors are more important. Why men seem to be more affected by income inequalities than women is a matter of speculation, but men and women may well be differently sensitive to different exposures.

Acknowledgments

I would like to thank Professor Denny Vågerö, Centre for Health Equity Studies at Stockholm University/Karolinska Institutet. I would also like to thank my colleagues at Stockholm Centre on Health of Societies in Transition, Södertörns Högskola for their help and critical reading.

REFERENCES

Footnotes

  • Funding: this research was funded by the Swedish Baltic Sea Foundation.

  • Conflicts of interest: none declared.

Linked Articles