Intended for healthcare professionals

Papers

Birth weight and cognitive function in young adult life: historical cohort study

BMJ 1997; 315 doi: https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.315.7105.401 (Published 16 August 1997) Cite this as: BMJ 1997;315:401
  1. Henrik Toft Sørensen, associate professora (HTS{at}soci.aau.dk),
  2. Svend Sabroe, associate professorb,
  3. Jørn Olsen, professorb,
  4. Kenneth J Rothman, professorc,
  5. Matthew W Gillman, associate professord,
  6. Peer Fischer, consultante
  1. a Department of Internal Medicine V, Aarhus University Hospital, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
  2. b Danish Epidemiology Science Centre, Department of Epidemiology and Social Medicine, University of Aarhus, 8000 Aarhus C, Denmark
  3. c Department of Epidemiology and Preventive Medicine, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, MA 02118, USA
  4. d Department of Ambulatory Care and Prevention, Harvard Medical School and Harvard Pilgrim Health Care, Boston, MA 02215, USA
  5. e Conscription Administration, County of North Jutland, 9000 Aalborg, Denmark
  1. Correspondence to: Dr Toft Sørensen
  • Accepted 19 May 1997

Abstract

Objective: To examine the relation between birth weight and cognitive function in young adult life.

Design: Retrospective cohort study based on birth registry data and cognitive function measured during evaluation for military service.

Subjects: 4300 Danish conscripts born between 1973 and 1975.

Main outcome measures: Mean score in the Boerge Prien test of cognitive function; score is the number of correct answers to 78 questions and correlates with full scale intelligence quotient (IQ).

Results: Mean score in the Boerge Prien test increased from 39.9 at a birth weight of ≤2500 g to 44.6 at a birth weight of 4200 g even after adjustment for gestational age and length at birth, maternal age and parity, and other variables. Above a birth weight of 4200 g the test score decreased slightly.

Conclusion: Birth weight is associated with cognitive performance in young adult life. Interference with fetal growth may influence adult cognitive performance.

Key messages

  • Low birth weight and proportionate smallness at birth have been associated with poorer cognitive function in early childhood

  • Only a few studies have examined whether this association persists into adult life

  • This study found that the mean cognitive test score in 4300 Danish conscripts increased up to a birth weight of 4200 g after adjustment for confounders; there was a slight decrease above a birth weight of 4200 g

  • Fetal growth seems to influence adult cognitive performance

  • If fetal growth has an impact on mental development it has important consequences from the perspective of maternal care

Introduction

Birth weight is associated with the development of coronary heart disease, stroke, and diabetes in adult life,1 and malnutrition in early life may affect the developing brain.2 3 However, few data exist on the link between birth weight and later cognitive function. One study found reduced mental development in early childhood in children with a low birth weight, especially if the placenta was normal.4 In contrast, a recent study reported little association between low birth weight and poor cognitive performance in adult life after adjustment for confounding factors.5 This study included 1576 men and women born between 1920 and 1943 in Britain, but only 47% of the number invited to participate took the cognitive function test.5

With such limited data further population based studies are needed; we therefore examined the relation between birth weight and cognitive performance in young men.

Subjects and methods

We conducted the study in the fifth conscription district of Denmark, which covers mainly the counties of North Jutland and Viborg. The population is about 700 000. Nearly all Danish men have to register with the draft board when they are about 18 and undergo physical and mental examination. We studied all men who were born in Denmark after 1 January 1973 who were drafted from and resided in the study area from 1 August 1993 to 31 July 1994.

All those drafted took a 45 minute validated group intelligence test, the Boerge Prien test, which was developed in 1957 for the Danish draft board.6 The test includes time limited subtests covering four categories of items: letter matrices, verbal analogies, number series, and geometric figures. The score is the total number of correct answers for 78 questions and is highly correlated with verbal intelligence quotient (IQ) (0.78), performance IQ (0.71), and full scale IQ (0.82) on the Wechsler adult intelligence scale.6 In the validation study the mean full scale IQ was 105.8 and the mean Boerge Prien test score 44.2.6

We linked the data on cognitive function to data from the Danish birth registry by means of personal identification numbers. Since 1968 all Danes have been given a 10 digit personal registration number at birth that is used in all Danish data sources, thus making linkage between registries simple and valid. The birth registry contains information on all births in Denmark since 1 January 1973. Data are obtained from the official reports filed by the midwives attending the delivery; in Denmark all deliveries are attended by a midwife. Birth weight is reported in categories of 250 g, and we used the midpoints of the categories in the analyses.

We first analysed the data by estimating the mean score in the Boerge Prien test according to birth weight and for each category of the variables listed in table 1). Since the relation was not linear we then used quadratic spline regression7 to illustrate the relation. In both analyses we adjusted for length at birth and gestational age and for maternal age, parity, marital status, education, and employment using the values in table 1 as categorised variables.

Table 1

Mean (SD) score in Boerge Prien test according to birth weight and other variables studied

View this table:

Results

A total of 5183 men were drafted during the study period. Of these, 4661 underwent a medical examination when drafted, 4300 of them having been born after the start of the computerised birth registry on 1 January 1973. A total of 522 subjects were excused from the examination because of asthma, osteochondrosis, or epilepsy, 505 of them having been born after 1 January 1973.

Mean birth weight in the 4300 subjects born after 1 January 1973 was 3471 (SD 0.54) g and mean length 52.2 (2.4) cm. The mean score in the Boerge Prien test was 43.1 (9.6). Table 1 shows mean scores according to the main study variables. The mean score increased with increasing birth weight and gestational age and with mother's age and occupational status. Figure 1 shows the relation between mean score and birth weight in the measured categories and by quadratic spline regression. Even after adjustment for multiple covariates the mean score in the Boerge Prien test increased with birth weight from 1900 g to 4200 g.

FIG 1
FIG 1

Estimated and observed mean score in Boerge Prien test of cognitive functioning according to birth weight. Smoothed curve was fitted by unrestricted quadratic spline regression with adjustment for gestational age and length at birth and for maternal age, parity, marital status, and employment

Discussion

In contrast to the findings of Martyn et al,5 we found positive associations between birth weight within the normal range and cognitive function 20 years later. The association between low birth weight and cognitive function corroborates previous findings.4 The reduced score in the Boerge Prien test for some men with high birth weights may plausibly be due to the increased risk of underlying diseases or birth traumas.

Olsen and Martyn et al controlled for social class,4 5 and Olsen also controlled for smoking, alcohol intake, and parents' school education.4 We also controlled for social factors but not for smoking or alcohol intake. Our population is large and without self selected non-responders. The study includes almost all young men in a well defined region. In the study by Martyn et al 47% of subjects did not respond.5 The risk of selective survival is probably higher in the English study because the subjects were born when the chances of survival of infants with low birth weight were much smaller than in our cohort. We had information on about 90% of the cohort. Selection bias seems, therefore, unlikely as explanation for the association.

Confounding is certainly a possible explanation for the findings. Our findings, as well as those of Martyn et al,5 may be confounded by parents' IQ, which we could not examine. Furthermore, we have no data on postnatal factors, such as nutrition8 9 and stimulation,10 that may influence cognitive function. Social class is much more homogeneous in Denmark than in Britain, but residual confounding is still possible despite adjustment for the employment and marital status of the mother.

An association between birth weight and cognitive function could be mediated through prenatal and postnatal factors. Lack of nutrition could cause disproportionate fetal growth, causing impaired brain development. A shortage of specific nutrients important for fetal growth and brain development could be a possible mechanism, and n-3 fatty acids may be the best candidate for such an effect. Variables that are important for cognitive functioning postnatally are breast feeding and early psychological stimuli, both of which could be related to birth weight.8 9 10

From the perspective of maternal care, it is important to know whether fetal growth has an impact on mental development. Our results support an association between birth weight and cognitive function in adult life, but data from other populations with careful control for confounding factors are needed.

Acknowledgments

Funding: Helsefonden (grant No 11/064-94). The activities of the Danish Epidemiology Science Centre are financed by a grant from the Danish National Research Foundation.

Conflict of interest: None.

References

  1. 1.
  2. 2.
  3. 3.
  4. 4.
  5. 5.
  6. 6.
  7. 7.
  8. 8.
  9. 9.
  10. 10.