Review
Going to the heart of the matter: do negative emotions cause coronary heart disease?

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-3999(99)00091-4Get rights and content

Abstract

Objective: Negative emotions, such as anger, anxiety, and depression, have emerged as potentially important risk factors for coronary heart disease. The purpose of this article is to consider the nature and function of emotions, to review epidemiological evidence for an association between the three negative emotions and coronary heart disease (CHD), to discuss briefly the mechanisms by which emotions may be linked to CHD, and to consider this evidence in light of theoretical insights provided by mainstream psychological research on emotions. Methods: We collected articles published between 1980 and 1998 on the relationship between each negative emotion and CHD. We also collected review articles or chapters published during the same time period that considered mechanisms by which emotions may increase CHD risk. We used a qualitative approach to review the published literature. Results: Evidence that anxiety is involved in the onset of CHD is strongest, whereas evidence for an association between anger and CHD is limited but suggestive. Although depression has consistently been linked to mortality following a myocardial infarction, evidence for its role in the onset of coronary disease is quite mixed. Numerous unresolved issues leave our current understanding of the emotion–health relationship incomplete. Psychological theories of emotion are considered to help address gaps in our knowledge. Conclusion: Growing evidence indicates that negative emotions may influence the development of CHD. The focused and specific consideration of negative emotions and their possible role in the etiology of CHD gives insight into current knowledge and suggests important directions for future research.

Introduction

“A mental disturbance provoking pain, excessive joy, hope or anxiety extends to the heart, where it affects its temper, and rate, impairing general nutrition and vigor” (from Harvey [1], p. 106). Coronary heart disease (CHD) is the leading cause of death in western, industrialized countries. Traditional risk factors (such as smoking, hypertension, hypercholesterolemia, obesity, and diabetes) explain only about 40% of the occurrence of CHD 2, 3. Although links between emotions and the heart have been hypothesized for centuries, until recently, convincing evidence for such relationships was lacking. Specific emotions like anxiety, anger, and depression have emerged as potentially important risk factors for CHD. 4, 5, 6, 7, 8, 9, 10, 11, 12 Among psychological factors potentially affecting the development of CHD, emotional states (e.g., anxiety, depression) are distinct from other psychological and behavioral factors like personality or coping style [13]. Emotions may influence cardiovascular health through multiple pathways. Specific pathways may involve excessive activation of the sympathetic nervous system, or the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocorticol (HPA) axis 14, 15. Other pathways may involve altered autonomic regulation of the heart 14, 16 or the promotion of atherosclerosis through indirect behavioral pathways (like smoking or high-fat diets) 17, 18, 19.

Evidence exists for associations of incident CHD with three specific negative emotions: anxiety; anger; and depression. These associations have often been considered under the broader umbrella of the relationship between CHD and psychosocial factors. However, a more focused consideration of negative emotions and their possible role in the etiology of CHD may suggest important directions for future research. The purpose of this article is first to consider the nature and function of emotions, to review the epidemiological evidence for an association between CHD and each negative emotion (anger, anxiety, and depression), to discuss briefly mechanisms by which these emotions may be linked to CHD, and finally to consider this evidence in light of theoretical insights provided by mainstream psychological research on emotions. Emotions can be a consequence of ill health and affect disease prognosis; however, in this review, we are primarily interested in considering whether emotions influence the development of CHD.

For the purposes of this review, we focus on research that directly addresses the relationship between negative emotion and CHD. Causality between emotion and cardiovascular health is bidirectional and effects accumulate over a long period of time. Cross-sectional and case–control studies have been used to examine this relationship; however, because of the preservation of the temporal order of the link between emotion and disease onset, more compelling evidence is provided by prospective studies that measure emotions among initially disease-free individuals. Although emotions are usually measured at a single point in time, the measurement approaches used in these studies are designed to identify the chronic nature of the emotion experience.

We conducted a literature search using the Medline and Psychological Abstracts data bases, and collected all articles published between 1980 and 1998 on the relationship between each negative emotion and CHD. Given their methodological advantage (i.e., reporting of emotions is not subject to recall bias), we placed greater emphasis on evidence obtained from large, prospective cohort studies. We also considered evidence that acute emotion states may trigger the onset of CHD. We further searched these data bases for review articles or chapters published during the same time period, which considered mechanisms by which emotions may increase risk of CHD. Although speculation on the links between negative emotions and CHD is not new [13], the number of studies providing evidence of a prospective association with any one of the emotions (anxiety, depression, or anger) is still limited. As we are interested in the effects of specific emotions, we felt combining studies across emotions in a formal pooling procedure to be inappropriate. Even when multiple studies were available for a single emotion (e.g., anxiety), they have tended to use quite disparate methods of assessment, such as assessing phobic anxiety versus a clinical diagnosis of panic disorder. For these reasons, formal meta-analytic techniques were not warranted, and instead, we opted for a more qualitative approach to reviewing the published literature [20]. We note that systematic reviews of the literature may still be biased due to the tendency for smaller studies with null findings not to be published (publication bias); thus, as with any review of published literature, our discussion of the literature should be viewed with appropriate caution.

Section snippets

Nature and function of emotion: brief overview

Emotions are comprised of cognitive, neurobiological, and behavioral components 21, 22, 23, 24. Specific emotions are a product of the interaction between the person and the environment, serving an adaptive function by mediating between continually changing situations and the individual's behavior 22, 24, 25. As such, emotions motivate an individual to respond to the environment, and are functionally appropriate processes that may nonetheless have dysfunctional consequences when the system is

Anger

Recent laboratory and epidemiological studies have suggested that high levels of anger increase the risk of CHD. Anger, along with hostility, has been implicated as a “toxic” component in the relationship between Type A Behavior Pattern and CHD 33, 36, 43, 44, 45, 46, 47, 48, 49, 50. Anger and hostility are strongly associated with each other; generally, anger is considered the emotion component of the broad hostility construct, whereas hostility is considered to be a long-standing attitudinal

Pathways from negative emotions to CHD

Emotions may be directly linked to cardiovascular disease because they activate the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenocortical (HPA) axis and the sympathetic–adrenal–medullary system (SAM), with resulting elevations in serum levels of cortisol and catecholamines [79]. Over time, recurring activation of these systems may set disease-related physiological processes in motion. For example, excessive or chronic emotional arousal may result in high levels of epinephrine/norepinephrine that in turn raise

Remaining questions

There are numerous unresolved issues that leave our current understanding of the emotion–health relationship incomplete (Table 2). A consideration of psychological theories of emotion may help to address a number of gaps in our knowledge. For example, the role of emotions in the onset versus the clinical course of CHD is not clearly understood. Although there is good evidence that depression adversely affects the prognosis of disease once it is established, its role in disease incidence is less

Conclusions

Growing evidence supports the hypothesis that negative emotions influence the development of CHD. Evidence is strongest for anxiety as a potential risk factor for CHD, whereas evidence for anger or depression is either more limited or more controversial. The convergence of findings across emotions despite the heterogeneity of measures employed and outcomes examined suggest that further consideration of these emotions as risk factors for CHD is clearly warranted. With the availability of

Acknowledgements

Dr. Kawachi was supported by a Career Development Award from the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute.

References (160)

  • M.G. Goldstein et al.

    Psychological factors affecting physical conditioncardiovascular disease literature review

    Psychosomatics

    (1992)
  • A.P. Haines et al.

    Phobic anxiety and ischaemic heart disease

    BMJ (Clin Res Ed)

    (1987)
  • I. Kawachi et al.

    Symptoms of anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease. The Normative Aging Study

    Circulation

    (1994)
  • I. Kawachi et al.

    Prospective study of phobic anxiety and risk of coronary heart disease in men

    Circulation

    (1994)
  • I. Kawachi et al.

    A prospective study of anger and coronary heart disease. The Normative Aging Study

    Circulation

    (1996)
  • L.D. Kubzansky et al.

    Is worrying bad for your heart? A prospective study of worry and coronary heart disease in the Normative Aging Study

    Circulation

    (1997)
  • L.D. Kubzansky et al.

    Anxiety and coronary heart diseasea synthesis of epidemiological, psychological, and experimental evidence

    Ann Behav Med

    (1998)
  • E.D. Eaker et al.

    Myocardial infarction and coronary death among womenPsychosocial predictors from a 20-year follow-up of women in The Framingham Study

    Am J Epidemiol

    (1992)
  • B. Dorian et al.

    Stress factors in the development of coronary artery disease

    J Occup Med

    (1984)
  • T. Kamarck et al.

    Biobehavioral factors in sudden cardiac death

    Psychol Bull

    (1991)
  • Schneiderman N. Psychophysiologic factors in atherogenesis and coronary artery disease. Circulation...
  • B.H. Friedman et al.

    Anxiety and autonomic flexibiltya cardiovascular approach

    Biol Psychol

    (1998)
  • T.W. Smith

    Hostility and healthcurrent status of a psychosomatic hypothesis

    Health Psychol

    (1992)
  • I.C. Siegler

    Hostility and riskdemographic and lifestyle variables

  • C. Hayward

    Psychiatric illness and cardiovascular disease risk

    Epidemiol Rev

    (1995)
  • J.C. Bailar

    The promise and problems of meta-analysis

    N Engl J Med

    (1997)
  • D.H. Barlow

    Anxiety and its disorders

    (1988)
  • K.R. Scherer

    Emotion as a processfunction, origin and regulation

    Soc Sci Inform

    (1982)
  • H. Leventhal et al.

    Emotion and illnessthe mind is in the body

  • N.H. Frijda

    The emotions

    (1986)
  • P.C. Ellsworth

    Some implications of cognitive appraisal theories of emotion

  • P.C. Ellsworth et al.

    From appraisal to emotiondifferences among unpleasant feelings

    Motiv Emot

    (1988)
  • N.H. Frijda

    Moods, emotions episodes, and emotions

  • R.S. Lazarus

    Progress on a cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion

    Am Psychologist

    (1991)
  • R.W. Levenson

    Human emotionsa functional view

  • R. Plutchik

    Emotiona psychoevolutionary synthesis

    (1980)
  • R.W. Levenson

    The search for autonomic specificity

  • R. Lazarus

    The stable and the unstable in emotion

  • T.Q. Miller et al.

    A meta-analytic review of research on hostility and physical health

    Psychol Bull

    (1996)
  • C.D. Spielberger et al.

    Stress and anxiety

    (1978)
  • C.D. Spielberger et al.

    The experience and expression of angerconstruction and validation of an anger expression scale

  • T.W. Smith et al.

    What's so unhealthy about hostility? Construct validity and psychosocial correlates of the Cook and Medley Ho scale

    Health Psychol

    (1985)
  • D.S. Goldstein

    Stress, catecholamines, and cardiovascular disease

    (1995)
  • S. Cohen et al.

    Strategies for measuring stress in studies of psychiatric and physical disorders

  • R.S. Lazarus

    Emotion and adaptation

    (1991)
  • T.D. Kemper

    Sociological models in the explanation of emotions

  • C.D. Spielberger

    State–Trait Anger Expression Inventory

    (1996)
  • J.P. Robinson et al.

    Measures of personality and social psychological attitudes

    (1991)
  • S.G. Haynes et al.

    Women, work, and coronary heart diseaseprospective findings from the Framingham heart study

    Am J Publ Health

    (1980)
  • K.A. Matthews

    Assessment of Type A behavior, anger, and hostility in epidemiological studies of cardiovascular disease

  • Cited by (342)

    • Role of Positive Emotions in Takotsubo Cardiomyopathy

      2023, Current Problems in Cardiology
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text