Elsevier

Social Science & Medicine

Volume 53, Issue 5, September 2001, Pages 603-614
Social Science & Medicine

Sense of coherence and school-related stress as predictors of subjective health complaints in early adolescence: interactive, indirect or direct relationships?

https://doi.org/10.1016/S0277-9536(00)00370-1Get rights and content

Abstract

The role of sense of coherence (SOC) on the relationship between adolescent school-related stress and subjective health complaints was tested with structural equation modelling. As part of the crossnational WHO-survey ‘Health behaviour in school-aged children 1997/98’ Norwegian representative samples of 1592 grade 6, 1534 grade 8, and 1605 grade 10 students completed measures on SOC, school-related stress and subjective health complaints. A test of nested structural models revealed that both stress-preventive (Δ χ2 814. 86, p<0.001), stress-moderating (Δ χ2 11.74, p<0.02) and main health-enhancing (Δ χ2 1289.1, p<0.001) effects of SOC were consistent with the data. A model including all these relationships fitted the data well (CFI=0.91, RMSEA=0.04). Age-group comparisons revealed that the association between SOC and stress grew weaker with age (p<0.05), whereas the direct association between SOC and health complaints grew stronger (p<0.001). The main effect of SOC accounted for between 39% (11 year olds) and 54% (15 year olds) of the variance in subjective health complaints. Findings indicate that SOC may potentially be a salutogenic factor in adolescents’ adaptation to school-related stress, and that relationships between SOC and healthy adaptation, may be evident in younger age-groups than previously anticipated.

Introduction

Subjective health complaints like headache, backache, and abdominal pains, are common in early adolescence (Aro, Paronen, & Aro, 1987; Garralda, 1996; Goodman & McGrath, 1991; King, Wold, Tudor-Smith, & Harel, 1996; Mikkelsson, Salminen, & Kautiainen, 1997). A series of studies have implicated school-related stress in the development and maintenance of such health complaints (Aro et al., 1987; Garralda, 1996; Hurrelmann, Engel, Holler, & Nordlohne, 1988; Ystgaard 1997). However, the finding that not all students develop complaints from school-related demands has directed the attention to factors that moderate the perception of stress, and the adverse health impact of stress (e.g. Wagner & Compas, 1990; Ystgaard, 1997). In adults, one of the stress moderators that has generated considerable interest is the sense of coherence (SOC), a global orientation to view life situations as comprehensible, manageable and meaningful (Antonovsky, 1987). In the original theoretical formulation Antonovsky (1987) proposed that SOC may influence stress and health in three ways: (1) SOC influences whether a stimuli is appraised as stressor or not; (2) SOC influences the extent to which a stressor leads to tension or not; and (3) SOC influences the extent to which tension leads to adverse health consequences. While research on adults in part support these assumptions (for a review, see Antonovsky, 1993), the role of SOC in child and adolescent health is largely unexplored. With the view that school adaptation has an essential impact on a wide range of social, psychological, and behavioural outcomes, empirical evidence on the stress moderating role of SOC during adolescence may offer particularly scope for development of prevention policies. The aim of the present paper is to examine the ways SOC and school-related stress interact in relation to subjective health complaints during early adolescence.

In the processes linking life situations to health, stress appraisal is the first process that SOC may influence. Stress research indicates that level of ambiguity and uncertainty are important dimensions in appraisals of life situations. Unpredictable or incomprehensible life situations are potent sources of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). As a global orientation to life, the sense of coherence (SOC), will influence the degree to which people view life demands as chaotic and incomprehensible, or coherent and comprehensible. Through the confidence that ‘…the stimuli deriving from one's internal and external environments are structured, predictable and explicable…‘ (Antonovsky, 1987, p. 19), individuals with a strong SOC will be less likely to perceive ambiguity in encounters with life demands.

In keeping with the hypothesis that a high SOC may help to appraise demands as non-stressful, studies on adult populations have reported moderate inverse associations between measures of SOC and measures of perceived stress. A review of these studies (Antonovsky, 1993) showed that that the associations are generally stronger for perceived measures of stress than for measures of stressful life events, suggesting a role in appraisal processes, and not in the actual exposure to stressful events.

As a next step in the stress process, SOC has been suggested to influence coping expectancies in encounters with stress (Antonovsky, 1987, p.19). According to the transactional model of stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984), coping expectancies develop from secondary appraisal processes, where people assess the means that are available to deal with the stressful condition. As a global orientation to life, individuals with a strong SOC will have a general confidence that resources are available to meet the demands posed by stressful situations (Antonovsky, 1987, p.19). This confidence increases the likelihood of positive coping expectancies. In related conceptual formulations (e.g. Bandura, 1986; Kobasa, 1979; Ursin, 1988) coping expectancies are assumed to moderate reactions to stress. In line with these models, Antonovsky (1987) proposes that a strong SOC may help to prevent stress from turning into potentially harmful tension. From this perspective SOC acts as a classic moderator of life stress.

Empirical studies on the stress-moderating role of SOC show mixed findings. In a study of Finnish adult workers, Feldt (1997) found that the relationship between work demands and health complaints was stronger for workers with a low SOC, but in statistical terms the interaction was weak. In a similar vein, Vahtera and colleagues (1996) found that job demands from active jobs lead to sickness spells in workers with low SOC, but not in workers with a high SOC. In contrast, a number of other studies have failed to detect stress-distress moderation (e.g. Anson, Carmel, Levenson, Bonneh, & Maoz, 1993; Flannery & Flannery, 1990), leaving the issue of SOC as a moderator unresolved.

As a third mechanism, Antonovsky suggested that a high SOC may prevent stress-associated tension from developing into health problems. Stressing the point that SOC is not a particular coping style, Antonovsky (1987) proposed that individuals with a high SOC are more likely to select the coping strategy that is efficient for dealing with the stressor. High SOC individuals tend to use problem-focused strategies, they are flexible in their choices of strategies, and they are skilled in using feedback to redirect coping attempts. As a consequence, individuals with a high SOC are, in general, more likely to remove the source of stress, and to terminate the associated tension. Over time, individuals with a strong SOC will experience shorter periods of harmful tension than individuals with a weak SOC, suggesting a main effect between level of SOC and health.

In line with the tension-termination hypothesis, a high SOC has been strongly associated with measures of self-reported health and well-being, as well as low scores on markers of disease (for a review see Antonovsky, 1993). While these findings are in line with the tension-termination hypothesis, authors have suggested that the strong associations to some extent may reflect methodological confounding between measures of SOC and measures of self-reported health (Geyer, 1997; Korotkov, 1993).

Through the influence on stress appraisals, coping expectancies and coping behaviour, SOC may affect processes that are essential in the development and maintenance of subjective health complaints. Uncertainty is a potent stimuli for the stress response. Once initiated, the profile of the stress response is moderated by response-outcome expectancies and control beliefs (Ursin & Hytten, 1992). Low perceived control over stressful conditions have been associated with a general tonic activation involving all biological response systems, including changes in neuro-endocrine, vegetative, neuro-muscular, central-nervous and immune system functioning (Ursin, 1997). Activation is sustained when coping efforts to remove the stressor are unsuccessful, or when no attempts are made to remove the stressor (i.e. helplessness). Sustained activation causes long term sensitisation of neural transmission (Antelman, Soares, & Gershon, 1997; Dubner & Ruda, 1992; Woolf & Thompson, 1991). Recent contributions view long-term sensitisation as a candidate mechanism for chronically elevated levels of health complaints (Ursin, 1997). In sum, these finding suggest that stressful appraisals, negative coping expectancies and unsuccessful coping behaviour are associated with physiological processes that may permanently lower the threshold for experiencing subjective health complaints.

In view of the pervasive impact that is claimed for the adult SOC, surprisingly little is known about the role of SOC in the normal adaptation of general adolescent populations. In the original theoretical formulation, Antonovsky (1987) emphasises that SOC is a developmental construct that becomes crystallised at the age of 30, suggesting a more fluctuating and less essential role for SOC in earlier age-groups: ‘The adolescent, at the very best can only have gained a tentative strong SOC, which may be useful for short-range prediction about coping with stressors and health status.’ (Antonovsky, 1987, p. 107).

The hypothesised limited role for SOC in adolescent health, has been paralleled by a limited research focus on these groups. Some authors view adolescent experiences as important for the development of SOC in adulthood (e.g. Lundberg, 1997; Cederblad, Dahlin, Hagnell, & Hansson, 1994) but few contributions have addressed the potential health impact SOC may have during adolescence. The few studies on child and adolescent SOC, have to a large degree focused on particular risk groups, such as adolescents experiencing evacuation stress (Antonovsky & Sagy, 1986), learning disabled children (Margalit & Efrati, 1996), and adolescents with chronic disease (Baker, 1998). Contrary to the idea of a ‘fluid and weak’ adolescent SOC, these studies indicate that a ‘young’ SOC may contribute to stress and coping in much the same way as does the ‘mature’ adult SOC.

While the above studies offer preliminary evidence that SOC may help adolescent risk groups to cope with particular difficult life conditions, the health impact of SOC may be even more far-reaching for normative demands that every adolescents encounter during the course of normal development. School-related demands are potent sources of stress in adolescent normal populations (Eme, Maisak, & Goodale, 1979; Greene, 1988; Henker, Whalen, & O’Neil, 1995). Elevated levels of such stress is associated with psychological distress (Wagner & Compas, 1990; Ystgaard, 1997) and somatic complaints (Aro et al., 1987; Garralda, 1996; Hurrelmann et al., 1988). As schooling is mandatory in most countries, exposure to school demands is beyond the control of adolescents. Identification of resources that may help to prevent stressful appraisals, or moderate the adverse health impact of stress, may serve as an important first step in developing preventive strategies.

Antonovsky's (1987) original contribution suggests that the adolescent SOC may affect level of health complaints indirectly by preventing school-related stress appraisals, interactively by moderating the impact of stress, and directly by reducing the likelihood of sustained activation. A potential shortcoming in previous research, is the failure to compare the relative importance of these mechanisms at given developmental stages in life. To gain more knowledge on the role of SOC in early adolescents’ adaptation to school-related stress, the present paper investigates each of these assumed relationships:

  • 1.

    SOC is inversely related to appraisals of school-related stress.

  • 2.

    SOC moderates the relationship between school-related stress on subjective health complaints.

  • 3.

    SOC is inversely related to health complaints.

Section snippets

Sample and sampling procedure

As part of the WHO survey ‘Health behaviour in school-aged children 1997/98’ a representative sample of 5026 Norwegian 11, 13 and 15 year olds took part in the study, representing a response rate of 78%. 1733 was from grade 6 (mean age 11.46), 1623 was from grade 8 (mean age 13.46), and 1670 was from grade 10 (mean age 15.48). The sample was obtained using a clustered sampling procedure with school-class as the sampling unit (Currie, 1998). Clustered samples may potentially underestimate

Results

Table 1 shows the correlation matrix and the means and standard deviations that was used for the estimation of the structural equations.

Discussion

In brief, we found that both stress-mediated effects, stress-interactive effects, and direct effect of SOC on health complaints were consistent with the data.

Conclusion

The aim of the present paper was to assess the role of SOC in adolescents health adaptation to school-related stress. The results provide some support for the general stress-health mechanisms that Antonovsky formulated. While the present study did not address stability of SOC, the strong cross-sectional consistency across samples, indirectly point to a degree of stability in SOC also in adolescence. As such the present study underscores the need to adopt a life-span perspective when examining

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Candace Currie for preparation of the research protocol of the ‘Health Behaviour in School-Aged Children 1997/98’ survey. The writing of the manuscript was made possible through a doctoral grant from the Norwegian Research Council, division of medicine and health.

References (53)

  • C.J Woolf et al.

    The induction and maintenance of central sensitisation is dependent on N-methyl-D-aspartatic acid receptor activationimplications for the treatment of post-injury pain hypersensitivity states

    Pain

    (1991)
  • O Anson et al.

    Coping with recent life eventsThe interplay of personal and collective resourcesit>

    Behavioral Medicine

    (1993)
  • S.M Antelman et al.

    Time-dependent sensitization-possible implications for clinical psychopharmacology

    Behavioural Pharmacology

    (1997)
  • A Antonovsky

    Unraveling the mystery of health. How people manage stress and stay well

    (1987)
  • A Antonovsky

    The structural sources of salutogenic strengths

  • H Antonovsky et al.

    The development of a sense of coherence and its impact on responses to stress situations

    The Journal of Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • H Aro et al.

    Psychosomatic symptoms among 14–16 year old Finnish adolescents

    Social Psychiatry

    (1987)
  • L.K Baker

    Sense of coherence in adolescents with cystic fibrosis

  • A Bandura

    Social foundations of thought and action

    (1986)
  • R.M Baron et al.

    The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological reasearchConceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • A.T Beck

    Cognition, affect, and psychopathology

  • P.M Bentler

    EQS structural equations program manual

    (1995)
  • J.R Busemeyer et al.

    Analysis of multiplicative combination rules when the causal variables are measured with error

    Psychological Bulletin

    (1983)
  • M Cederblad et al.

    Salutogenic childhood factors reported by middle-aged individualsFollow-up of the children experiencing three or more childhood psychiatric risk factorsit>

    European Archives of Clinical Neuroscience

    (1994)
  • Currie, C. (1998). Health behaviour in school-aged children. Research protocol for the 1997/98 Survey. University of...
  • R Eme et al.

    Seriousness of adolescent problems

    Adolescence

    (1979)
  • Cited by (124)

    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text